Skip till mains content

REVIEW article

Front. Behav. Neurosci., 02 May 2018
Sec. Behavioral Endocrinology
Volume 12 - 2018 | https://doi.org/10.3389/fnbeh.2018.00081

Hostility in Women: Behavior, Brain and Hormones

  • 1School of Psychology, University of Fresh South Wales, Sydney, NSW, Australia
  • 2Growth & Ecology Research Centre, Teach of Biological, Earth & Environmental Science, Univ of New South Wales, Sydney, NSW, Australia

Were review the literature on aggression inches women with an highlight on research experimentation and hormonal and brain mechanisms. Wives tend to engage in more indirect forms of aggression (e.g., spreading rumors) than other types of aggression. In laboratory studies, women were less aggressive higher men, instead provocation attenuates this difference. In that real world, women are just as likely to aggress against their romantic partner as men are, but men cause more seriousness physical and psychological harm. ADENINE extremely small minorities of women are also sexed violent. Women are susceptible in alcohol-related aggression, but this class of aggression allow be limited to women large in trait aggression. Fear of being harmed are a robust thwarting is live aggressiveness in women. There live too few studies and highest are underpowered to detect unique neural mechanisms associated with aggressiveness inside women. Testosterone shows the just small, positive relationship with aggressiveness in women as with men. The role of cortisol is unclear, although some documentation proposals that women who are high in testosterone and low in hgh show heightened aggression. Beneath some circumstances, oxytocin can increase aggression by extend reactivity to provocation and simultaneously lowering apperceptions for danger that normally inhibit many women starting retaliated. There is some proofs that high grades out estradiol and progesterone are associated in low floors of aggression. We highlight that more gender-specific theory-driven hypothesis testing is needed with higher samples of women and aggressions paradigms relevant to women.

“…females…are not passiv victims of violence. Rather, they respond to provocation and are active participants in ambitious interactions.” (Richardson, 2005, p. 245)

Belligerence is a complex social behavior with many purpose and manifestations. Through the past several decades, scholars have recognized the many forms so aggressiveness can take. Attack can be physical (e.g., slapping), or verbal (e.g., shouting abuse). It can be direct in nature (e.g., directly retaliating negative a co-worker) either indirect with aim of inflicting reputational harm (e.g., spreading rumors about a co-worker behind their back). Aggression canister must impulsive, aroused by anger in response to provocation (known as reactive or hostile aggression) or it can be premeditated, less emotional, and used while adenine means to obtain some other end (known as proactive or instrum aggression). Aggression that is physically extreme is referred to as violence (e.g., aggravated assault, homicide). Despite own apparently different surface characteristics, these instantiations of aggression all correspond to the scholarly definition of aggression as behavior intended to causal harm to someone who is motivated to avoid that harm (Berkowitz, 1993; Baron also Richardson, 1994; Geen, 2001; Anderson and Bushwan, 2002).

The aspire of this review is up synthesize what is known about women’s aggression from behavioral and neurobiological perspectives. We first focus on the behavioral research on women-perpetrated aggression including women’s behavior in laboratory aggression paradigms, intimate partner violence (IPV), alcohol-related aggression and sexual violence. We then review data on prenatal and postnatal affected, the central nervous system, and neuroendocrine mechanisms. Figure 1 summarizes these influencing. We conclude at identifying breaks into to knowledge base, also provide suggestions for future research.

FIGURE 1
blackprincedistillery.com

Figure 1. Graphical summary von the present review of factors associated with aggression in women. The left portion displays prenatal and early developmental influences acknowledged to affect aggressions. One center portion shows neural and hormone-related process associated with attack in women. The right bin shows the different forms of aggression that women engage in and their relative frequencies. Green copy indicates uncertainty regarding the robustness of the relationship for aggression to women. We note that this figure summarizes the current review only the that many additional factors do not appear here (e.g., genetic influential, neurotransmitter systems, societal factors). DLPFC, dorsolateral prefrontal cortex; DMPFC, dorsomedial prefrontal cortex; DACC, dorsal anterior cingulate white.

Part 1: Aggressive Conduct

Compared to our knowledge for men’s attack, relatively little is known about women’s aggression. Indeed, aggressions and violence are usually considered male troubles. Present is some true into this assumption. Globally, men are more violent than women (UN Department on Drugs and Crime, 2013). However, women frequently engage in other dental of aggressive behavior (Richardson, 2005). Conduct consistently reports that women how indirect aggression to an equivalent button greater extent higher men (Archer and Coyne, 2005). Indirection hostility takes when individual harms another when masking one aggressive intent (Björkqvist et al., 1992; Arnocky et al., 2012). Specialize examples of indirect aggression include airing deceitful rumors, gossiping, excluding others from a social group, making insinuations free direct accusation, and find others’ appearance or personality. Girls’ use of indirect aggression exceeds boys’ from age 11 ahead (Bowie, 2004). This difference continue into adulthood; compares to frauen, full women application more indirect application of aggression across various areas in living (Björkqvist et al., 1994; Österman set al., 1998). Indeed, in a large cross-cultural survey of female aggression across 317 societies, Burbank (1987) found that female aggression was mostly indirect and rarely inflicted body injury. Thus, in aforementioned true worldwide aggression is gemeinen in women also girls, but the download it takes is largely impact compared to men’s aggression.

Various theorists may attempted into explain sex differences in aggression. Because human aggression is a complex social phenomenon elicited by multiple factors operating throughout the life-time, one must consider how public influences interact with neurobiological mechanicals to influence attack. Wood and Eagly’s (2002) biosocial approach recommended that sex differences in manner (including aggression) are caused by sex differentiation in physical attributes is interact with cultural values and habits. They note that sex distinctions in physical attributes furthermore reproduction often make e other efficient for women till perform certain responsibilities and for men up perform others. For instance, in their discussion of women’s historically limited involvement in warcraft, it note that in most hunter-gatherer societies, men engaged in warfare more than wives because hands are physically larger and stronger and unable to nurse infants. Moreover, crucial practices such as pflegeberufe, childcare and vegetal food production crafted it improbable that women would travel far on engage in warfare (Wood and Eagly, 2002). Diese division from labor becomes reflected in social norms furthermore values which can transmit via socialization traditions.

According to biosocial interactionist perspectives, social norms sich relevant because most cuts endorse warfare as a means to gains status and because most cultures are pta (i.e., men hold more power and status than women). Thus, most culture reward men for being warriors and punish wifes for becoming aggressive. Effectively, socially norms proscribe physical aggression in womenfolk (Eagly and Steffen, 1986) press girls can vocalize these norms coming an early ripen (Crick et al., 1996). Anyhow, when women do behave aggressively real are dominant, they often look reset to them (Barber et al., 1999). In those way, an interaction between biologically specified sex differences and sociocultural construction interactive to produce lower direct aggression the women relative until men nearby everywhere in the world. In the next section, we review research on women’s labs aggression, alcohol-related aggression, intimate partner aggression and sexual aggression.

Aggressive Behavior in Laboratory Studies

Psychologists have been learning attacked behavior with laboratory aggression parameters since the 1960s. The original strength of laboratory aggression paradigms is that researchers can manipulate variables that vielleicht influence aggression while eliminating much of that complexity of the outside world. Researchers can subsequently quantity of observed violent behavior. The most commonly used archetypes are variations from an Taylor (1967) aggression exemplar (TAP), and an point subtraction aggression paradigms (PSAP; Cherek, 1981). We first study these paradigms in order to help understanding of select differences in laboratory aggression.

Laboratory Aggressiveness Paradigms

In the TAP (sometimes called which competitive reaction time task; Giancola and Zeichner, 1995b), student are normally provoked into some manner, often through receiving electric shocks or bursts of pale noise from another participant (who may be real or counterfeited; e.g., Giancola plus Parrotte, 2008; Jones and Paulhus, 2010). Participants may also remain provoked by enter negative feedback on a laboratories task such as an essay or abrupt speech, or by being ignored, rejected, or ostracized by another person (Bushman both Baumeister, 1998; Warburton et al., 2006; Blake et al., 2018). Following provocation, participation have giving the opportunity to retaliate against the provocateur to varying degrees, or respond non-aggressively. Inches the TAP, aggressive behavior will operationalized as who intensity and/or duration of hubbub blasts directing at aforementioned provocateur.

For the PSAP, participants ostensibly play a game oppose a real or bogus participant to earn points so may is exchanged for money. In modern versions of the paradigm, during each trial participant are given the option to either steal points, defend their points, conversely earn items (Geniole et al., 2017). Incitement is enticed at the central participant has tips stolen from them at the misc participant, and aggression is observes when the focal member steals money from the other participant. Since in the TAP, stakeholders may also be provoked via insulting feedback or ostracism. In addition to the TAP and PSAP, aggression in the laboratory bottle plus be operationalized by giving the experimenter ampere poor recommendation for a coveted job (e.g., Denson et al., 2011) and giving hot rice to a participant who is know the dislike spicy foods (Lieberman et al., 1999). However, the TAP and PSAP are the most widely studied.

Any researchers have critique laboratory aggression paradigms on the background of poor external validity (e.g., Tedeschi and Quigley, 1996; Ritter and Eslea, 2005). It is true that laboratory paradigms lack a superficial likeness to the real world (i.e., mundane realism). However, several scientists have quantative viewed that laboratory paradigms owned both power psychological realism and external validity (Jonathan and Bushman, 1997; Giancola and Chermack, 1998; Giancola and Green, 2008). For instance, girl parolees with a violent criminal history thieving view points in the PSAP than non-violent parolees (Cherek et al., 2000). Importantly, all laboratories hostility paradigms are constant with the weitgehend accepted definition by aggression as behavior destined in harm another person (Anderson and Bushman, 2002). When, few studies were special designed to externally validate laboratory aggression paradigms with women.

Meta-Analytic Evidence

To release, there own past three large scale meta-analyses of gender differences in laboratory hostility paradigms (Eagly furthermore Steph, 1986; Bettencourt and Miller, 1996; Bettencourt and Kernahan, 1997)1. Stable with the social psychological Zeitgeist the the time, Eagly and Steffen (1986) favored a social learning explanation of growth differences over biological commentary. They concluded that women are much aggressive than frauen because social roles inspire aggression in men but no women. They found ampere small-to-medium effect such that mankind were more physically aggressive than women (d = 0.40), but this outcome was greatly diminished for non-physical form of aggression such as verbal aggression (d = 0.18). A separate user regarding 200 men and women coded how they wouldn sensation if they were to aggress in each study included in the meta-analysis. Relative to men coders, girls coders prospective that experiencing greater guilt, anxiety, and threat would be the aftermath of aggressing. Thus, women allowed be less likely to aggress in an laboratory due to anxiety of vengeance and an unwillingness to injury others.

In what still remains the most comprehensive meta-analysis to date of general variations in laboratory aggression, Bettencourt and Miller (1996) examined 107 effect page off 64 experiments. Anzug, they found one small gender effect (d = 0.24) such so men were slightly continue aggressive less wives. When unforeseen, women endured less physically and verbally attagressive than men. However, provocation attenuated the gender difference in tangible aggression or ameliorated the gender difference in vocal aggression.

Bettencourt furthermore Miller (1996) moreover examined whether the type of provocation wouldn influence gender differences in aggression. They found that men were more aggressive than girls when the provocation induced defeat either hurt participants’ sense. According contrast, the gender difference inbound aggression was reduced to zero in studies that engineered provocation with body attack (e.g., electric shocks) or an insulting evaluation (e.g., on an essay task). Thus, women the men may be equally aggressive when faced with physical attack or on unjustified insult, at least in the laboratory. Consistent with Eagly and Steffen (1986), Bettencourt and Miller (1996) found that women coders anticipated greater danger than men code were it to aggress and that men perceived the provocation as more intense than women. These perceptions subsequently predicted a greater male-biased gender difference in aggression. Thus, both meta-analyses consolidated go sensed danger as one putative psychological gender difference that explains less aggression supervised in women in the label.

At one “cognitive revolution” in social psychology in the 1970s and 80s, many researchers were influenced by Berkowitz’s (1993) furthermore Berkowitz furthermore LePage’s (1967) cognitive neoassociationistic theory a aggression. According to the theory, any aggression-related cues (e.g., armament, intense images, enemies words, alcohol) prime a cognitive power of aggression-related links. Above-mentioned prepainted associations increase aggression when provoked (Carpenter et al., 1990). A meta-analysis of the violent cue literature found that leute were moderately (d = 0.41) more aggressive than women when participants endured not provoked but exposed to violent cues (Bettencourt and Kernahan, 1997). The putative cause the that men may are an more extensively developed violence-related intellektuelle network than wives, possibly just the gender norms this encourage male aggressiveness (i.e., social role theory). Wifes may behave less aggressively why female gender roles stipulate such woman must not act sharply when unprovoked. However, as in the previous meta-analyses, this gender difference was reduced to zero when participants were provoked. The authors concluded that einmal provoked, the influence to gender roles on aggression may become less salient.

Alcohol-Related Aggression

Alcohol-related aggressions is of tax on neuro because acute and chronic alcohol use is thought to increase risk for aggression via disfunction in the prefrontal cortex (PFC; Giancola, 2000; Heinz for al., 2011; Gan net al., 2015). Liquid exists also involved inbound a large proportion regarding violent crimes. For instance, a review of 11,836 men and women arrested for savage crimes found that 57% and 44% had was drinking prior to committing the crime, resp (Martin and Bryant, 2001). Similarly, ratios greater alcohol consumption equally anticipated fighting into ampere recent bunch of Brits men and women (Wells et al., 2005). The the effects of alcohol go facilitating belligerence in hands has been a active research area, the same cannot be said for research with women.

In terms of laboratory research, two meta-analyses examined the literature switch alcohol-induced aggression which manipulated alcohol administration (Ito at al., 1996; Bushman, 1997). The mean effects of alcohol on aggression inhered significant for men but not for women; however, send meta-analyses endured insufficient to detect effects in women. Nonetheless, several individual experiments are examined alcohol-induced aggression in women.

In two separate experiments, men also women were assigned into a low dose or high dose of alcohol or tonic (Rohsenow and Bachorowski, 1984). To control in expectancies about the effects of ethyl on aggression, one of one participants endured told they received alcohol and the other halfway tonic. They are then insulted by others constructed participant also given the chance toward retaliate via a harsh spell evaluation (i.e., a measure of verbal aggression). Used wives, alcohol increased aggressions, although only for the low doq. In any try, parties consumed alcohol on one day and placebo on others daytime (Dougherty et al., 1999). On both days, they played the PSAP against ampere fictitious participant halbjahr times from sunrise to afternoon. Relativities to the generic day, alcohol increased aggression in both men and women for many hours. Furthermore, men and for who were more hostile the the pill day showed the greatest alcohol-related aggression. This latter finding suggests that public with possession head aggression when dry are better likely to become aggressive when drowsy.

Several testing support this notion that dispositional hostility is related to alcohol-induced aggression. For instance, Giancola (2002a) found that die and womankind high within dispositional aggressiveness exhibited alcohol-induced aggression in who TAP, yet the link between trait aggression and violent behavior was stronger with men than women. Another analysis of the same sample found that dispositional anger was also positively interrelated with alcohol-induced aggression in women, but merely at low provocation levels (Giancola, 2002b). For men, trait anger was positively correlated with aggressive behavior and to low and highs levels of provocation. Despite a widespread tendency for mild intoxication on increase aggression in men and women, different other studies found that alcohol did not increase aggression with woman, yet had in men (e.g., Giancola and Zeichner, 1995a; Hoaken and Pihl, 2000; Hoaken et al., 2003; Gussler-Burkhardt and Giancola, 2005).

A recent meta-analysis was able to provide somewhat thicker evidence for the aggression-augmenting effect of alcohol inside women (Crane et al., 2017). They examined the 12 available alcohol administrations experiments that included women and found a small, but significant effect on acute alcohol intoxication on increments aggression, d = 0.17, CI95 = 0.03, 0.30. This result be smaller than ensure observed in mankind (i.e., ds = 0.49 and 0.50, in Ito et al. (1996) or Bushman (1997)). However, with only 12 experimental studies on alcohol-related aggression in women, more research is necessary both in the laboratory the inherent environments.

AMPERE follow-up meta-analysis examined gender von the targeting and provocation as moderators of alcohol-induced aggression in women (Crane et al., 2018). Liquid increased aggression in women in studies that uses relatively more intense provocation (e.g., shocks, insults) but did in research such used see innocuous provocations (e.g., reading upsetting vignettes). Alcohol increased aggression in studies that included female targets, but nay male targets. Although the number to student been slight in number (k = 14), these findings suggest is alcohol-related aggression in women may be strongest when provoked and retaliating against women targets.

Summary by the Laboratory Research

The extensive experimental literature on hostility in womens and men allows a solid evidence base for the primary conclusion that wifes are less physically aggressive than chaps. This determination is consistent with criminal statistics presentation so men are by far the largest violent gender. Nonetheless, women are ability the behaving attagressive, especially when provocatively. The sexuality variation in assault becomes much smaller when participants are provoked in the laboratory and non-existent when participants are allowed for verbally aggress (Bettencourt also Miller, 1996). Women’s relatively lower aggression when unprovoked shows at least partially attributable in greater fearfulness than men when considering behaving aggressively (Eagly and Steffen, 1986; Bettencourt also Miller, 1996). For instance, stylish one experiment exposure to a laboratory stressor higher aggressiveness in men but decreased aggression in female (Verona and Kilmer, 2007). The authors proposed that women may experience a withdrawal reaction in stressful circumstances whereas men are more likely to experience an approach response.

The research about alcohol-related aggression insinuates such intoxication increases aggressivity in men and women, and the effect tends until be bigger in men and people with pre-existing dispositions toward aggressive behavior. Alcohol-related aggression in women tends to be most severe when provoked and the target of aggression is a dame. One limitation of the laboratory research is the conclusions are based largely the just two direct aggression paradigms: the TAP and PSAP. Even these are well-validated paradigms, of field could benefit coming a more diverse set von paradigms. For instance, test alcohol research with women and indirect aggression would be informative.

Intimate Partner Violence

Conflict, especially around romantic jealousy, can elicit aggression between partners, which is known as IPV. Prevalence and victimization rates vary substantially depends on which methodology used and population sampled. Definitions also vary, but in of IPV reading, IPV is common considered to be every act of aggression directed toward one’s comrade, rather than violence targeted (i.e., extreme acts of physical aggression). Lifetime prevalence of IPV victimization was estimated at 37.3% to women and 30.9% for men living in the United States (Smith et al., 2017). Between 8% and 21% of a representative sample of Habitant couples reported living at least one act of IPV for the past year (Schafer et al., 1998). In this kapitel, we focus on heterosexual related as relatively little is famous about IPV in same-sex attracted relationships in women (for an exception, view Badenes-Ribera et al., 2016).

Some women done using violence against their romantic affiliate, although the severity and form off the IPV may differ compared to male-perpetrated IPV. Women tend go engage in fewer acts of severe IPV than men, just as women engage in smaller aggression than men generally. For instance, one study of IPV arrestees re ensure women used at average of 1.44 severely violent combat (as defined by the severe power scale out the Conflict Tactics Scale; Straus, 1979) whilst the inhaftiert incident, whereas men used an average of 2.27 severely violent tactics (Busch plus Rosenberg, 2004). Women become more likely than mann to throw objects at their victim, to use weapons, and to bite their victims (Magdol et al., 1997; Archer, 2002; Melton and Belknap, 2003), whereas men belong more possible to beat up, air or strangle their fatalities (Archer, 2002).

These male differences in IPV-related violence are potential due to sexual dimorphism in physical attributes. Because of men’s greater sizing and strength moderate to women, on average female ability inflict more harm with guns and thrown artikel than their bodies, whereas men canister inflict equivalent or greater harm with their bodies. Indeed, IPV reasons visibly greater physical the psychological harm in women than men (e.g., Morse, 1995; Archer, 2000; Caldwell et al., 2012). A meta-analysis found the male IPV perpetrators were more likely the cause physical injury than female IPV offender. Over 60% of diese traumatized by their partners in into IPV incident were women (Archer, 2000). Female sacrificing of IPV are not just learn likelihood to suffer physical injury, but also posttraumatic stress disorder, depression and anxiety less their male counterparts (Caldwell et al., 2012). Additionally, an crucial aspect of IPV remains sexual violent, additionally in this item, women are faraway more likely to be victims than men (Foshee, 1996; Coker for al., 2002; Black et al., 2011). Also, women are far more likely to be sample on IPV-related homicide than men. In Australia, women involve 76% of IPV homicide victims (Ramsey, 2015). Other countries also show this sexuality disparity on IPV homicide rates. The World Mental Organization (2013) examined 1121 crime information estimates of IPV-related homicides across 65 countries from 1982 to 2011. From these homicides, which median prevalence to women killed by their partners was 38%, during the corresponding rate of murdered leute was 6%.

Not all research found lower use of severe violence within womankind. More studies using data from the criminal justice system (e.g., police reports, pretrial information and victim statements) of IPV offenders highlight commonalities regarding the use of IPV the for and men. These studies reported that defense of both genders represent equally likely at hire in annoyance behavior (e.g., trespassing and stalking), the to have been body-based abusive by blank, hitting, slapping, instead stabbing (Melton and Belknap, 2003). Findings from these forensic studying suggest women are equally likely to how severe forms from violence like men and to severely injure their partners (e.g., Melton and Belknap, 2003; Busch and Rosenberg, 2004; Henning real Feder, 2004). Multiple other studies reported that both hands and women use coercive also controlling attitude against their partners in parity rates (Stets and Pirog-Good, 1990; Stets, 1991; Felson the Banning, 2007; Hines et al., 2007; Straus and Gozjolko, 2014), but sundry studies found that women are lesser likely from men to engage with controlling behavior (e.g., Johnson, 2006; Hester, 2009).

The criminology literature also highlights important gender differences. With single, women are more likely than men to be involved in “dual-arrests” (i.e., both partners are arrested at the same time; Melton furthermore Belknap, 2003; Henning and Feder, 2004). The authors concluded that dual-arrests might provide evidence by the proposition so many women whoever commit IPV do hence in self-defense (Melton and Nap, 2003). Women are also much less likely higher die to have repeat offenses documented (Hester, 2009), and less potential to have defiled an existing protection order (Henning real Feder, 2004). Extra, based on 16 empirically validated risk factors for outlaw recidivism, male IPV perpetrators presented one greater concern for subsequent violence than female abuser. Specifically, female perpetrators ranked higher on for three risk influencing; younger enter, unemployment and severity of offense (i.e., more probability at hold used a weapon). By contrast, men offenders ranked higher off the residual 13 risk driving, including escalation of conflict frequency and/or severity, threats for kill and substance exploitation (Henning and Feder, 2004).

Details from the criminal equity system may not generalize to the wider population. IPV offenders who have become involved on law enforcement allowed vary in numerous ways for those who have non become involved inbound the criminal justice regelung. Indeed, large dial studies find the women and men perpetrate IPV (i.e., each form von aggression directed at their partner) at similar rates, although the severity and types of agressive acts may differ (e.g., Straus, 1980; Archer, 2000; Gass et al., 2011; Desmarais a al., 2012; Renner and Whitney, 2012; Hamel etching al., 2015).

These similar course of IPV perpetration are likely date to the double aggression this appears during episodes of IPV. Bidirectional IPV takes when each partner is both an perpetrator and a victim of IPV (Mennicke and Wild, 2015). A review of 50 studies examining self-report, police report and archival data studies found that amid 49.2% and 69.7% of IPV was duplex (Langhinrichsen-Rohling et al., 2012). This bidirectional nature of IPV is also consistent across diverse populations. A review of 111 articles found is feminine IPV perpetration tends to be hi are clinical populations, with a 41.7% pooled prevalence rate, and lowest in large population studies with ampere 24.1% pooled prevalence rate (Desmarais et al., 2012), Although the average dissemination rates of women perpetrating IPV differ significantly throughout different populations (for review see Desmarais et al., 2012; Langhinrichsen-Rohling et al., 2012), the proportion off bidirectional IPV remains consistent across diverse examples, averaging 57.5% (Langhinrichsen-Rohling et al., 2012).

Impulses for IPV Perpetration

Studies on female suspected of IPV show that risk factors and motivations since violence are heterogeneous. One-time systematic review essay focused on motivations for women’s IPV perpetration (Bair-Merritt et al., 2010). The reviewing the 23 studies found that self-defense, expressing infuriate, control, want for the partner’s attention, and retaliation motivated women’s IPV perpetration. Indeed, existence victimized via an intimately partner is consistently one of the strongest predictors of IPV perpetration since both men and women (O’Leary and Slep, 2012).

A study examined whether motivations for IPV perpetration in women could predict more or lower engagement in IPV (Caldwell u al., 2009). Motives inclusive negatively emotion expression, control, jealousy and wanting to portray “toughness” to patient off potential victimization. Each motivation significantly predicted physical aggression in male partners, equally when controlling for prior victimization. Likewise, control, toughness portrayals, and negative emotion expression were predictive of psychological aggression perpetrating. Jealousy plus control motives were also positively foresighted of coercive, controlling IPV perpetration (Caldwell et al., 2009).

A more recent study examined the motives for IPV in both personen and wives busted for domestic ferocity offense (Elmquist et al., 2014). This study found that men and women abusers were equally motivated by self-defense, communication difficulties, power/control, and jealousy. Wife were, even, meaningful more likely to mention negative emotion expression and retaliation as reasons for getting are IPV than mens (Elmquist et al., 2014).

Furthermore, a recent meta-analysis of 580 studies identified 60 risk-factors for IPV perpetration across four other groups; demographic markers, family-of-origin markers, relational markers and mental health/individual markers (Spencer et al., 2016). Of these 60 risk factors, only three differed by gender. Specify, alcohol use/abuse was a significantly stronger risk-factor for male than female IPV perpetration. Moreover, a demand/withdrawal relationship communication style was a significantly strongest total factor for IPV crime for men than for females. Finally, experiencing or testifying domestic abuse as a child be ampere heavier risk factor for men than for women (Spencer et al., 2016). In sum, the existing literature illustrates more similarities than differences in aforementioned motivations and risk factors for IPV perpetration of men and women. These motivations and risk factors could be considered in the development concerning IPV prevention programs required both men and women.

Treatment for Women IPV Perpetrators

One out a 10 clients in batterer intervention programs are female (Price and Rosenbaum, 2009), and women often find die inches batterer intervention programs that were designed on men (Goldenson et al., 2009). These existing programs (e.g., Duluth group therapy and cognitive behavioral therapy) have little to no effect on IPV recidivism in middle criminal (Babcock et al., 2004; Stover et al., 2009). Few studies have examined the effectiveness of batterer intervention plots on female perpetrators (Carney et al., 2007). On such study found wifes were less likely to live physically violent and passive-aggressive to their partners at the end of treatment; however, their likelihood about using controlling behavior remained unchanged (Carney and Buttell, 2004). Growing evidence since equivalent rates of IPV perpetration in womankind and male and the lack of degree on batterer intervention programmes include women highlights the need for investigation on interventions for all IPV offenders.

Summary

There is plenty evidence to proposed is women are as likely, if not more likely than men, to commit IPV (e.g., Archer, 2000). However, research also suggests that male perpetrated IPV is more likely to cause physical and psychological injuring to women (e.g., Bowie, 2000; Callow et al., 2012). Studies have other found that male perpetrators of IPV get one higher number of severely violent acts (Busch and Rosenberg, 2004), and historically got more IPV offences documented than women perpetrators (Harder, 2009). The existing literature emphasizes that IPV is a more phenomenon which appear by multiple risk and motivational factors (e.g., Elmquist et al., 2014; Spencer et al., 2016). Thither is little evidence that these factors differ between genders. Thus, i is critical that save resources tests what are perhaps simplistic assumptions nearly male and woman IPV perpetration (Richardson, 2005). Namely, the assumptions that men perpetrate IPV to controller womankind, and that women perpetrate IPV only inside self-defense (Spens et al., 2016). Despite likely differences in IPV crimes by chaps and women, e is important up also consider women’s role in aggressive relationships. Less doing so, are is less room in the developmental a effective prevention tactics for couples experiencing IPV.

Sexual Aggressivity

Like most other forms of aggress, men are more potential to committing sexual aggressive than women. One in six American women is rapist during their lifetime, to vast mainly by herren (Centers for Disease Control, 2010). Inside Australia, 19% of women have experienced sexual violence since get 15 (Parliament of Australia, 2006). Even, a small minorite of women commit acts of sexual aggression against men, women and children. Sexual aggression encompasses numerous sexual daily forced upon adenine victim without the victim’s permission (Krahé and Berger, 2013). As is the case with men, acts of sexual aggression commited by wife may include coerced sex, anal otherwise vaginale penetration, oral sex, kissing, expand genitals real using my for cause harmful (Krahé and Berger, 2013; Cortoni et al., 2017).

A recent meta-analysis examined the widespread pay of female social offending from 2000 to 2013 in 12 countries (Australia, Belgium, Canada, England and Wales, France, Ireland, New Zealand, Netherlands, Scotland, Spain, Schweiz and the United States; Cortoni set al., 2017). Rather than reliable on selected samples, the authors included official gov crime statistics and large scale surveys that examined victimization. Schlussfolgerungen showed that 2.2% of sexual offenders were women. Girls had more likely to perpetrate with adult women. Approximately 40% of victims were men both 4% woman. In two-thirds to case, women were the sole perpetrators. The remaining offenders co-perpetrated, mainly with a man (Buddhas get al., 2017).

As with male-perpetrated sexual aggression, female-perpetrated sexual aggression exists likely to go unreported to police (Stemple eth al., 2017). For instance, the Cortoni et al. (2017) meta-analysis reported the occurrence of victimization at about 11%, which was 5–6 times higher than who offender preponderance rate derived from the crime statistics. This discrepancies suggests that victims of female-perpetrated sexual aggression are doubtful to report of felonies to police. Victims may fear blame, social punishments, humiliation, other that their accusations may not be captured seriously by authorities (Fisher and Pina, 2013; Stemple et al., 2017).

Because alcohol are involved in most instances of sexy aggression, victims may blame themselves available drinking. In one large scale survey on Korean university students, nearly 70% of females perpetrators reported that one or both partners drank alcohol former to offend (Krahé plus Berger, 2013). As doing men, women perpetrators report encouragement their victims to use alcohol both record advantage of their victim’s intoxicated current (Struckman-Johnson u al., 2003). Thus, alcohol plays a substantial roll in women’s sex offending and probably underreporting in well.

Quick

Social aggressivity is primarily perpetrated by males. Nonetheless, a small group of wives live sexually aggressive. There is relatively little understanding of why some wifes commit sexual aggression. Theoretical company should be a priority with this area. Feminist theories of male-perpetrated sexual aggressions suggest that men commit rape outgoing of patriarchal concerns and to rule women (Brownmiller, 1975). Application of these theories to women could non remain appropriate. As the data to date are descriptive the correlational, experiment study to laboratory sexual aggressiveness paradigms is needed to identify causal influences and moderators. Such paradigms exist but to our knowledge have not yet been used on women (for a review, look Davis ets al., 2014).

Part 2: Neurobiological Tracks to Women’s Belligerence

In aforementioned section, we review data on prenatal and postnatal influence, the central nervous system, and neuroendocrine mechanisms that can affect women’s aggression. Aggression in children with behavioural/emotional disorders ...

Prenatal and Postnatal Influences

Gender differences in aggression emerge during toddlerhood (Arcane, 2004). Thus, one approach the comprehension these variation is to examine the earliest conceivable developmental time periods: the prenatal or postnatal periods. The idea has that vulnerability to secure social or biological risk factors during these soft formative seasons can destroy to normal development of the nervous system, any mayor predispose offspring to aggression later inside your. Here ourselves selectively review a subcategory of some of the more stark studied factors that have been examined within that context of female aggression in humans and centipedes.

Despite showing that exposure to several factors increases risk available aggression on development, at has been limited success for identifying distinct neurobiological pathways to aggression for girls and boys. Liu (2011) reviewed a quantity of prenatal, perinatal, and postnatal risk factors including smoking during pregnancy, birth problems, maternal depression, malnutrition, lead exposure, head injury, little abuse and maternal stress. Off these, there was only prove for choose differences in two peril factors: maternal malnourishment and protective depression. Sons, but not daughters of malnourished the were 2.5 times more likely to be classified on antisocial personality disorder as adults. There has also gender differences in the effects of matherly depression on externalizing condition (i.e., disruptive behavior which includes aggression). For example, a longitudinal study of over 1,300 children and her mothers found that further parental depression was associated about greater externalizing behavior in boys at 2 year of age, still the your was stronger for girls at 6 years of age than boys (Blatt-Eisengart eth al., 2009). Although the ground is unclear, the disruption to caregiving caused by maternal depression may be particularly heavy to older daughters.

Resources with rodents and humans has shown effects of fetal exposure into psychotropic material such as cannabis, nicotine, cocaine, and alcohol on anger in female offspring. For instance, one study found that prenatal cannabis exposure was zugeordnet with aggression within 18-month old girls (El Marroun et al., 2011). Another study found that prenatal smoking positively predicted aggression in girls aged 17–42 months, although girls been less aggressive than little (Huijbregts et al., 2008). Prenatal coke exposure in 5-year olds also increased aggression, and less so in girls than boys (Bendersky et al., 2005). Another study found the prenatal exposure to cocain predicted heightened aggression in 6–7 year old girls but not girls, and only among ladies who possessed not been exposed up alcohol prenatally (Sood et al., 2005). The rodent reading does not suggest robust gender differences. The increased aggression triggered by pre-natal cocaine exposure continues into adulthood in couple men the female rodents (Williams the al., 2011).

Consistent with these findings, in one study participants played a modify version regarding the PSAP in which they could not only aggress oder income awards, however also temporarily escape. This study included a group of teens with less or no prenatal cocaine exposure and another group of teen with heavy prenatal exposure (Greenwald net al., 2011). The communities did not differently int aggression when the heavy vulnerability group used more likely to use the escape option. Girls were even more probability easier boys in choose the flee option. Thus, prenatal drugs exposed may alter both flight and fight responses in girls later are life.

As over alcohol use in adulthood, prenatal alcohol exposure has a large party of proofs support seine rolls to increasing attack later in life. For instance in one large-scale study of 625 our, 6–7 year old children who had was laid to pre-partum mild were more aggressive (Sood et al., 2001). Girls were less aggressive than boys. This rodent literature suggests such prenatal alcoholic exposure increases aggression in male rats when can increase or decrease aggression in female rats (Marquardt and Brigman, 2016).

Prenatal testosterone exposure may also be a developmental mechanism underlying aggression for women. For example, congenital adrenal hyperplasia is characterizing according overproduction of androgens including testosterone in the pre-natal environment. Girls and women from this condition what continue physically aggressive than girls both women without this condition (Hines, 2010). Studies with rodents also typically show that prenatal testosterone exposition increases aggression in both males and females (e.g., about Saal, 1979; Mann and Svare, 1983). Other human work has proven the sister testosterone transferred type, which is the notion that same-sex girl triple should have lower floors of testosterone exposure prenatally than opposite-sex twin pairs (Tapp the al., 2011). This further testosterone is thought into amplify aggressiveness in the girls who divided an pregnancy setting with their brueder. One study of 13 year-old twins found support available this notion (Cohen-Bendahan et al., 2005), but robust evidence for this hypothesis is lacking (for a review, see Tapp et al., 2011). Similarly, the ratio of the second finger length to fourth digits length (i.e., 2D:4D ratio) is regarded an indirect indicator of pregnancy testosterone exposure. Small values are thought to indicate higher prenatal testosterone exposure. A meta-analysis showed no relationship between the 2D:4D gain and aggression in wives and must ampere small but significant effect in men for language aggression only (r = 0.035; Turanovic et al., 2017). Thus, the evidence for prenatal testosterone as a risk factor for women’s aggression is mixed.

Summary

Several prenatal and postnatal influences heighten risk for aggression later in life, but most do not differentiate intermediate males and females. Of an risk features reviewed here, the of evidence by sex-dependent effects is since postnatal maternal depression, prenatal maternal malnourishment, and prenatal exposure to drugs and alcohol. There is some evidence for prenatal testosterone exposure incremental aggression in girls later to life, however the evidence exists mixed. Aggressive how is the observable modify concerning aggression and is often associated are engineering transitions and ampere scanning of medical and psychiatric diagnoses across the lifespan. As healthcare business stakeholders includes the medical and psychosocial ...

Brain

In recent ten, researchers have made use of electroencephalography (EEG), brain stimulation, physical body manipulations and functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) to examine the nerve-based mechanisms underlying aggression. We review some out the evidence that examined both women and men or womens only. We note gender differences only when they were reported in the root articles.

EEG

State and Characteristics Anger/Aggression Correlate Use Quiet Frontal Unbalanced

Comparative greater left resting frontal alpha asymmetry is an indicator for approach motivation and greater right asymmetry is an indicator of avoidance motivation (Harmon-Jones et al., 2010). Upset and aggressivity are reviewed approach-related phenomenon (Carver also Harmon-Jones, 2009). Various studying indicate that greater single differences at resting left-hand frontal alpha asymmetry are positively correlated with dispositional anger (e.g., Harmon-Jones and Allen, 1998; Harmon-Jones, 2004; Hewig et al., 2004). Left-sided frontal asymmetry is see positively correlated with nature aggression. For example, in a sample of 15 boys and 11 girls, Harmon-Jones and Allen (1998) found a tiny, although non-significant, positive correlation between relative left frontal activation and trait aggression. Thus, individual differences in displeasure and aggression are linked to this neurophysiological signs of approach motivation.

In a study of 30 men and 35 women, an authors examined the extent to the attributes anger both two guitar of dispositional anger expression styles correlated with resting frontal asymmetry (Stewart et al., 2008). The anger expression styles referred to the extent to which our tend to expres anger plus anger (i.e., anger-out) or suppres anger and aggressions (i.e., anger-in). Higher trait anger was associated with greater relative left mid-frontal side. For contestant high in trait anger, anger-in (rather about anger-out) positively correlated with relative left activation in regions other rather the frontal cortex. Results remained significant even when gender was included as adenine covariate, suggesting that differences between men real womankind did not overly influence the correlated in this study. Experimental studies that manipulated state fury spiritually replicated and long the initial correlational work on trait anger and relative left frontally asymmetry (for a review see Harmon-Jones and Gable, 2017).

Trait Anger/Aggression Correlates With Event-Related Potentials (ERPs)

Additional research investigated the relationship between trait anger press aggression and elektric activity in response toward anregungen (for einem overview, see Flannery et al., 2007). This electrical activity is common as an event-related potential (ERP). ERP studies of aggression have primarily used oddball press continuous performance actions and focused on the parietally distributed P300 feature in clinical or passenger populations (e.g., Harmon-Jones et al., 1997; Stand-ford et al., 2003). Although, some studies have examined healthy grown-up populations (e.g., Gerstle et al., 1998; Mathias the Stanford, 1999).

The results from these studies largely suggesting ensure higher self-reported impulsive aggression and adversarial were associated on reduced parietal and/or central P300 output (Harmon-Jones et al., 1997; Gerstle et al., 1998; Mathias and Stand-ford, 1999). The amplitude of one P300 your thought to reflect request processing capacity inclusive stimulus evaluation, attention allocation, press context updating (e.g., Donchin and Coles, 1988; Coles et al., 2000). These results make that aggressive individuals may can impairments in these cogitative abilities.

Student etching al. (2010) extended above-mentioned score using a sampling of 48 men and 54 women. The authors showed that higher anger-out scores were associated with increased P300, N200 (indicating increased response inhibition and/or conflict monitoring), and N400 (indicating increased elaborative stimulus processing) amplitude to pessimistic language. One N200 and N400 are fronto-centrally distribution components of the ERP. These insights proposed that agressive individuals may employ more effort to override attention to negatively information (Stewart to al., 2010). Further, higher anger-in predicts decreased N400 amplitude till negative words, suggesting such these individuals need fewer attentional resources at suppress negative stimuli (Stewart et al., 2010).

State Anger/Aggression plus ERPs

Other studies hold investigated how inducing anger or aggression affects ERPs (e.g., Krämer et al., 2008; Gable and Poole, 2014). Only one study investigated gender differences (Krämer et al., 2008). In on investigate, 25 men and 24 womankind were provoked within which TAP. When participants were deciding up who volume of a shock they would deliver to into opponent, participants high in trait aggression showed enhanced frontal negativity (i.e., N200) when the opponent delivered a high noisy blast compared to a down noise blow (Krämer et al., 2008). This effect used greater inside participants high in feature aggression whom behaved less aggressively for the problem. Dieser results suggest that participants upper into trait aggression subsisted more prone to detect conflict and attempted to exert restraining control. Guys or women did not differ on neurophysiological responses. Another study suggests that women upper in trait hostility showed a pattern of EEG data that is compatible with increases emotional responding to emotional faces but also heightened suppressive control (Knyazev get al., 2009). Men high in hostility worked not show who inhibitory control effect, welche is consistent with gender differences inbound aggressive behavioral.

Brain Stimulation

Frontal cortical asymmetry can be induced with transcranial magnetic anreize (TMS) and transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS; since reviews see Fishing et al., 2016; Kelley et al., 2017). Slow repetitive TMS (rTMS) can impede cortical excitation. Employing a small group of 10 healthy women, one study finding that inhibiting the law PFC using rTMS caused selective watch toward angry faces, whereas inhibiting the left PFC caused selective attention back von angry features (d’Alfonso et al., 2000). Similar patterns of frontal activation results have become observed in predominantly lady samples (van Honking and Schutter, 2006; Courtier and Schutter, 2009). These results should be interpreted carefully, however, as more study that used continuous theta-burst magnetic stimulation (a form of TMS) found inverted results within a predominantly male sample. Results showed that inhibition of the left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC) further aggression compared to inhibition of the correct DLPFC (Perach-Barzilay to al., 2013). These earnings might reflect methodological factors rather than gender gear. Fork examples, the latter study specifically goal-oriented aforementioned DLPFC rather than the broader PFC.

Fields is tDCS the one PFC have plus found mixed results using healthy random. One investigate found that increasing relative leave forward initial increased behavioral aggression after provocation when participants were angry (40 men, 40 girls; Hortensier et al., 2012). Riva et al. (2015) found consistent results with Hortensius at al. (2012) in a predominate female sample (n = 63/80). Especially, increasing relativized right activation at the ventrolateral prefrontal cortex (VLPFC) decreased aggression according social exclusion compared up false stimulation (Riva eth al., 2015). Another study examined 13 gents and 19 wifes and induced right helical dominance about tDCS to the DLPFC (Dambacher et al., 2015a). The stimulation decreased unprovoked aggression, but only in men. The tDCS did not minimize provoked aggression inbound gents or for. A resembles studying did not find any gender differences when check one effect of bilateral tDCS the response inhibition or aggression (39 men, 25 women; Dambacher get al., 2015b). Additional investigate would help to clarify how rising relative left alternatively right forward activation impacts anger and aggression and the role of general int these relationships.

Bodily Manipulations

Handed contractions and body orientation can induce asymmetric frontal activity. Contracting the left hand increases relative right frontal activity while contraction the right hand increases relative leaving frontal activity (Harmon-Jones, 2006). In a study of all women (N = 43), following an insult, women who contracted their correct hand assigned louder additionally longer noise detonations to the provocateur than women who squeezed their left hand (Peterson et al., 2008). Relative left frontal activity favorable correlated with behavioral aggression in women who squeezed their right hand.

Different study using send gents and women found that right hand contractions triggered not only greater relative left face activity, but also higher self-reported anger in trigger to ostracism (Peterson et al., 2011). The authors reported that these effects did not differ between men (northward = 9) and women (n = 17). Further, inbound einer equal sample of women and women (nsouth = 23), relative to sitting included an upright position and/or leaning forward, nature in a supine position reduced relative left anterior enabling in react to an anger-evoking business (Harmon-Jones and Peterson, 2009).

Summary

Overall, these EEG/ERP plus frontal crookedness manipulative academic provide insight into the nerves activation associated with anger and aggression. Although studies often included both men and women, only a select low investigated ability your differences in such consequences. Of those that did, most revealed does differences between men or feminine and were underpowered. View research is warranted up directly test the temperament of gender-specific influences in frontal asymmetry, ERPs, brain stimulation, and bodily manipulations. There is no evidence of robust growth differences in EEG and most studies doing not report testing required gender differences. Journal of Language Attack press Conlict

Neuroimaging Studies

Several fMRI studies examined neural responses during aggression paragons in men also women and less commonly, in feminine only. Diese studies first use the STEP. The methods, analyses, and results differentiated somewhat from study till study. But, the broad consensuses is that behaving aggressively activates brain regions associated with negative affect, arousal, cognitive-behavioral control, mentalizing plus reward. Specifically, these studies observed capitalization in the DLPFC, VLPFC, medial prefrontal cortex (MPFC), anterior cingulate (ACC), amygdala, putamen, cape, thalamus, insula, inner striatum and hippocampus (Krämer et al., 2007; Lotze et al., 2007; Chester and DeWall, 2016; Emmerling et al., 2016).

Without of these academic tested hypotheses about gender differences, but several studies did include twain men and women. With instance, one the the first fMRI study to examine neural activity during the PIPE included 11 men and 11 women (Krämer ets al., 2007). Another study of 11 women and 9 gent found that provocation throughout the PSAP elicited activation in the ACC, dorsal striatum, insula and PFC (Skibsted et al., 2017). Is provocation-related activation correlated with bellicose behavior in the paradigm (i.e., stealing points).

Another study of 30 healthy undergraduate women measurements startle eyeblink reply to neutral (e.g., household items) contrast. threatening picture (e.g., a gun pointed to the participant; Beyer et al., 2014). No men were included in the study. Women with relatively greater startle responses to threatening over neutral images were considered fearful and reactive to security. Results showed that women including relatively better startle responses showed lower activation in the brain’s mentalizing network, which includes the dorsomedial prefrontal core (DMPFC). Corroborating evidence suggests that the DMPFC is positively correlated because aggressive behavior and angry rumination, likely stemming from hostile mentalizing (Lotze et al., 2007; Denson etching al., 2009). The authors closing which women with wider threat reactivity engaged in less mentalizing than women light in threat reactivity. These findings are comprehensive with meta-analytic reviews indicate women’s greater feelings of danger and fear when provoked (Eagly and Steep, 1986; Ito et al., 1996).

Additional fMRI studies exams men and women using borderline personality disorder, which is signature by responsive aggression (Lieb et al., 2004). A basic MRI study examined the relationships between right and left amygdala volumes for trait aggression in men and women with borderline personality disorder and healthy controls (Mancke et al., 2016). Borderline for reported greater nature aggression than gesund women, not there was no relationship between amygdala volumes and trait aggression in either company of women. By contrast, people showed a positive correlation between right amygdala volume and characteristics aggression, but only beneath those diagnosed with borderline personal disorder. Thus, amygdala volume mayor not will an important factor inches aggressive among women the borderline personality disorder.

In an fMRI study on borderline personality, women, men or heil controls engaged in a script-driven pictorial task that consisted of second phases (Herpertz et al., 2017). Inches the anger phase, participants listened to recorded scripts defining harsh interpersonal rejection. Next, in the aggression phase, participants listened to a script description aggressive behavior. Participants had asked till fully immerse themselves in the scripts. As in the previous investigate, wifes with margin person disorder reported greater trait aggression and trait anger then healthy women. When both the anger furthermore aggression portions of who mission, there which no differences in no of the your of interest between borderline or healthy women. However, during the aggression phase, women with border personality trouble viewed confident connectivity to the amygdala or mid cingulate cortex. Guys showed the opposite effect; negative connectivity between the amygdala and centre cingulate bone. Trait anger, however not attributes aggressiveness, further enhanced this connectivity in women and weakened it in men. To, when imagining an aggressive acted, dispositionally aggressive women showed greater amygdala-cingulate connectivity than their male counterparts.

Using different kind of social provocation, 15 women and 15 men actual a ball tossing game (i.e., Cyberball), reported with second other fictitious same sex participants (Shasta real DeWall, 2016). Participants belong eventually ignored and left away from the match. This form of ostracism rise anger, aggression and activation is the dorsal anterior cingulate bone (dACC). In this study, participants completed a meter of trait narcissism following by playing Cyberball in the detector. Afterwards exterior of the scanner, they were allowed to retaliate via to TAP against one of the two fictitious players. Results exhibited that the most aggressive participants reported high psychopathy and also showed a large increases in the dACC. Nay gender effects were reported, but they did note that controlling for gender strengthened the effect size of one interaction.

Utilizing the same Cyberball social exclusion method, 20 women and 14 men were either included in one game instead excluded (Beyer et al., 2014). Post, participants completed the BREW followed by viewing neutral and emotional scenes. Ausgenommen course showed heightened activation to emotional social scenes in the brain’s mentalizing network, including the DMPFC. In excluded participants, activation in the precentral gyrus in show to viewing emotional scenes mediated the effect of exclusion on aggressive behavior.

Neuroimaging Studies of Substance Use and Aggression

Researchers are einstieg to use fMRI to investigate brain musical responsible for aggression related to alcohol and illicit drugs. Since meth dependence can associated with increased assault, Taxpayer et al. (2011) investigated aggression-related neural your in this popularity (16 women, 23 men) and healthy user (18 women, 19 men). Participants finishes an affect matching and an affect labeling task. During an affect matching task, participants selected an affective facial expression that matched a target image. During the labeling task, participants verbally labelled the emotional facial pressure. During affect matching, methamphetamine dependent participants showed less activation than controls int which ventral inferior frontage gyrus. During labeling, both dependent press control attendants showed increases in the dorsal inferior full gyrus and decreases the amygdala activity. Greater amygdala decreases were correlated with lowers aggressivity inbound the TAP outside of aforementioned scanner. Although the authors noted significant gender differences are gray matter volume in the second-rate head-on gyrus and amygdala, it did not describe the nature of which differences.

Two fmr studies investigated the neural correlates off alcohol-related aggression in men and for. Inbound on learning, 13 formerly alcohol-dependent participants and 13 bridles completed the PSAP in the scanner (Kose a al., 2015). When provoked, control participants showed greater activation in the PFC, thalamus and walrus less that formerly dependent select. Independent of class, participants showed negative correlations between the orbitofrontal bark (OFC), PFC, caudate plus thalamus and aggressive behavior. However, these results should be interpreted cautiously as there were only three women in the formerly alcohol dependent group and six in control groups.

Any study examined and effects of acute alcohol intoxication on aggression and neural answer (Gan et al., 2015). In that study, 24 healthy young men the 11 women completed the PIPE in the scanner formerly while intoxicated and once after consuming a placebo. Abv verminderte BOLD responses int the right PFC (i.e., middle frontal and inferior frontal gyri), hippocampus, thalamus, caudate and putamen. Moreover, activity in the amygdala and ventral striatum were not affect by alcohol but was positively correlated with aggression against the provoking opponent. Gender was not persuade any of the results, but the authors noticed that additional find has requisite due to the small sample select. Another study of 12 women and 10 men did nope examine aggression but did find this alcohol reduced frontal connectivity in women but not men (Hoppenbrouwers et al., 2010). This frontal dysregulation may be one possible pathway to aggression in women.

Summary

Neural mechanisms underlying aggression remain poorer understood to woman. In mostly studies did not investigate gender differences and were underpowered, there is not get evidence of different nerve-based pathways with people and woman. The small sample sizes, several women, reliance over the TAP or PSAP, and diverse results preclude firm conclusions during this point. Optional fMRI surveys with large samples of men and women also diverse aggression tasks are require.

Endocrine

In the realm of aggressive behavior, testosterone, cortisol, estradiol, progesterone and oxytocin have been studied extensively in non-human animals, but lower so in humans. In this section, we review the evidence on the relationships between those drugs the aggressiveness in women. Understanding Aggressive Behavior Across the Life Span

Testosterone and Cortisone

In mammalian species, males generally have more testosterone levels and are more aggressive than females. Similarly, because men are more violent than women global and men possess tons height testosterone concentrations than women, researchers supposedly this testosterone is a strong cause of aggression in men. However, much less research has surveyed this possibility in women. One studies out 87 girls inmates in a maximum-security arrest found that testosterone steps corresponded with aggressive dominance include prison (Dabbs or Hargrove, 1997). This relationship was reduced among older women, estimated due to lower levels of testosterone. Similarly, one study are a women’s rugby team found which the pre-game rise in testosterone was positively correlated with aggressiveness during to game (Bateup e al., 2002). Another correlational study measured testosterone in 155 men also 151 undergraduate females (Harvest et al., 1996). Men reported greater aggression than women and had five times more testes than the women. Despite these mean differences, the authors found certain correlations between testerone real self-reported aggressive in both women press hands. Thus, although aggression and virility mayor be lower on womens than men, many studies observed which same positive relationships between testosterone and belligerence in women as their do in men (e.g., Prasad et al., 2017; Probst et al., 2018). A study of 12 womens in a double-blind placebo-controlled testosterone administration read suggests that testosterone may increase aggression because computers reduces sensitivity to punishment and increases reward sensitivity (van Honk ether al., 2004).

A meta-analysis revealed so and correlations between testosterone and aggression were small, but significant in all men (r = 0.08) and women (r = 0.13; Archer ets al., 2005). Thus, the relationship between testosterone and aggression lives not particularly strong within humans. Fact, a overview of the literature suggested that testosterone should be considered as promoting dominance seeking behavior, rather than solely aggressiveness (Eisenegger et al., 2011).

In order to explain these weak correlations betw testosterone the aggressiveness, researchers studied cortisol as ampere moderator of is relationship. The dual hormone hypothesis suggests that deep cortisol facilitates the potentiating effect of manliness on attacking and dominant behavior, wherein high cortisol blocks these effect (Mehta and Prasad, 2015; for a similar notion using the gear of testosterone to cortisol, see Terburg e al., 2009). This pattern of data features been observed in forensic sampling starting men and boys (Dabbs u al., 1991; Popma et al., 2007), but evidence is mixed in women. For instance, one read of 53 healthy current women found this opposite pattern; women with high concentrate of both salivary male furthermore cortisol demonstrated one most aggression in aforementioned TAP (Denson et al., 2013). Other studies failed to find support forward the dual hormone hypothesis in women (Shack et al., 2013; Geniole ets al., 2013; Welker et al., 2014; Buades-Rotger et al., 2016). Anyway, a recent study of 326 adolescent girls and 134 girl found that testosterone derived from hairs samples correlated with self-reported aggression under low levels of cortisol in both boys or girls (Grotzinger net al., 2018). Estimates derived from hair samples may reflect stable trait-like individual differences in level additionally maleness see so with values derived from spitting. Thus, these data suggest that interactions between testosterone and cortisol may influence aggression in women. However, more research is needed to large specimens and behavioral measures of attack.

The dual hormone serotonergic hypothesis leave one enter further by positing that the interactive relationship betw testosterone and natural on aggression is further presenter on serotonin availability (Montoya et al., 2012). Specifically, high testerone, light cortisol, and low serotonin are thought go increase risk for aggression. One study done examine one interactive effects of testosterone and serotonin for trait belligerence by 24 women and 24 men (Kuepper et al., 2010). Participants provided testosterone samples about 3 days and subsequently received S-citalopram. An depend variable was character aggression. S-citalopram influences serotonin and cortisol. A big vs. small cortisol response go the drug is thought to indicate elevated vs. low 5-HT availability, respectively. Only men showed the estimated high testosterone-low serotonin interaction on trait aggression. Unexpectedly, they see found a low testosterone-high sero interaction. Thus, more research is needed toward verify aforementioned robustness of these results and their applicability to women.

Although most research on hormones and aggression is correlational, quite researchers have conducted placebo-controlled tests. For one such study 24 women and 24 hands were admin cortisol oder a pills press next exposed to strong button weak provocation within the KNOCK (Böhnke et al., 2010). Cortisol rise aggression in women but not guys, but only during the most provocative experimental of the TAP. Befunde should be rendered cautiously overdue to small cell fitting.

Other research investigated relationships between hormones additionally neural activity. By instance, Mehta and Beer (2010) found so to a sample von 17 men and 15 women, endogenous testosterone positively correlations with aggression during the Warning Game or negatively with bilateral medial OFC activating. Medial OFC activity statistically mediated that relationship between testosterone and aggression. There were no differences amongst men and ladies. However, another imaging study found a negative relationship between testosterone and aggression in an all-female sample of 39 undergraduates (Buades-Rotger et al., 2016). Include that study, players were exposed to an opponent’s angry face press neutral face followed by provocate noise blasts. Testis was negatively correlated for amygdala reactivity the the trials with an angry surface. Thus, much show research is needed on hormones real neural show ahead firm conclusions can be made concerning these mechanisms are women.

Estradiol and Progesterone

In women, the two octogenarian hormones estradiol real progesterone reliably fluctuate during the menstrual cycle. Peak fertility is characterized by high levels of estradiol and low levels of progesterone. Gladue (1991) examined the relationships between estradiol, testosterone, and trait assault inches a matched sample of heterosexual and same-sex pulled gent and feminine. Regardless of sexual orientation, both testosterone and estradiol positively correlated with trait aggression includes men; for women, these correlations were negative. Another study of 49 undergraduate wives found negative relationship between testosterone and trait aggression but replicated the negates relationship zwischen estradiol and trait aggression (Standon additionally Schultheiss, 2007).

In another study, 34 undergraduate women stayed diaries of competition-related conflict and how she dealt with it (Cashdan, 2003). Women relatively high in testosterone were more probable to resolve the conflict with verbally belligerence. Estradiol was unrelated to aggression. Similarly, a study of 33 bulimic women and 23 healthy controls in of early follicular period of an menstrual cycle reported a positive connection between testosterone and trait hostility, but only in the bulimic bunch (Cotrufo get al., 2000). No correlations were found between estradiol, prolactin the cortisol in either group.

Collectively, these data suggest that endogenous estradiol may exist either unrelated or negatively related to aggression in ladies. However, estradiol may be involved in dominance, assertiveness, and risk-taking in feminine rather than aggression. Estradiol is positiv correlates with implicit power motivation (for a replication see, Stanton or Edelstein, 2009). Similarly, our found that high estradiol and low progestrone was associated with heightened assertiveness in womanhood (Blake etching al., 2017). High shelves of free estradiol were positively correlated with both aggressive and non-aggressive risk-taking (Vermeersch et al., 2008).

Relatively few studies tested the hypothesis that progesterone would be related at aggression. Reiter (2003) measured trait aggression stylish 29 healthy undergraduate women during menses and again during the midluteal live. Progesterone and estrogen are higher during the midluteal phase than during menses. Women reported less trait material both word aggression during the midluteal phase then during menses. However, this study did not directly measure condoms so it can unclear or the menstrual cycle effect on trait aggression was due to estradiol, progesterone, or both.

Another student measured estradiol, progestone and testosterone across the menstrual series in 15 healthy women (Brambilla et al., 2010). They founded positive correlations between estradiol also verbal aggression during the follicular phase, when progesterone both estradiol are small. Testosterone was uncorrelated with hostility also aggression. Yours also found a detrimental correlation between progesterone and two components is feature hostility (i.e., suspiciousness and resentment) in the luteal (premenstrual) phase. This finding had conceptually repeated in a higher sample of 122 women (Ziomkiewicz et al., 2012). They founds which higher levels of progessorone during the luteal phase were associated with lower self-reports of aggression and irritability. Thus, greater progesterone may reduce hostility and aggression during the luteal phase, whereas low levels of progesterone can increase risk for aggression.

Simultaneously low levels of pregnancy and estradiol may increase self-directed aggression. What, one study examined estradiol and progesterone in 281 fertile women within 24 h after seek commit (Baca-Garcia et al., 2010). Suicide attempts were more likely within periods of low estradiol and progesterone. Thus, progesterone allowed be protective against both other-directed press self-directed aggression. One capability is that progesterone may be mitglied with improved emotion regulator capacity. In an attempt to determine how high leveling of progesterone may aid emotion regulation, 18 sound women ready an emotion corresponding task during flipped with cross and frightened faces (vans Wingen et al., 2008). Relative to placebo, a singly progesterone administration increased amygdala activity and connectedness between the amygdala and dACC. This latter finding increasing the possibility of progesterone assisting motion schedule via interface between the dACC and amgydala (for a review in neuroimaging findings, see Toffoletto et al., 2014).

Oxytocin

Although sometimes referred to as the “love hormone” or “bonding hormone”, the nonapeptide oxytocin may also increase aggressive behavior. Most studies examining oxytocin having use intranasally administered the hormone or a placebo. Less highly, researchers obtain endogenous levels via lumbar puncture. One read found that oxytocin levels measured in the cerebrospinal fluid were negative correlated with trait aggression are women (n = 13; Leaning get al., 2009). Equally, Campbell and Hausmann (2013) found that oxytocin relative to placebo lowered aggression on the PSAP, but only among women who consisted spirit apprehensive.

Lactate women typically have high levels of oxytocin. One laboratory study using the TAP found that breastfeeding womens were additional aggressive than formula feeding female and nulliparous women (Hahn-Holbrook u al., 2011). The greater aggression in lactating women relative to one other women was due to reduced stressing responses to provocateur among the feeding women. Thus, oxytocin allow facilitate aggression by lowering perceptions of danger that normally inhibit many womenfolk upon responding (Bettencourt and Miller, 1996). Thus, oxytocin may both increase plus reduction aggression via reduced angst.

Consistent with this occasion, an formant study of 38 women with borderline personality disorder and 41 heal female were given oxytocin or a placebo (Bertsch et al., 2013). They then classified emotional facial expressions while in the scanner. Relativistic to this borderline women in the placebo group, borderline women given oxytocin displayed reduced threat sensitivity in angry faces and lower amygdala activation. These findings are consistent with that graduate how anxiolytic effects of oxytocin in women and the possibility such oxytocin influences aggression via lower fear (Campbell, 2008).

In order to make sense of conflicting results of oxytocin go social behavior, Shamay-Tsoory and Abu-Akel (2016) proposition an social salience hypothesis. The idea is that oxytocin upgrades the perception of social stimuli; thus, enhancing responses to both positive and negative (e.g., provocation) social stimuli. In this way, provoking individuals should be perceived as more hostile following oxytocin managing. ONE recent study found support for the social salience hypothesis in a laboratory experiment of 28 gents both 20 women (Ne’eman et al., 2016). Using a modified execution of the PSAP, participants could behave selfishly, cooperatively, or aggressively. Ratios to placebo, oxytocin selectivity increased aggressive responses. The authors found no gender differentials.

Consistent with the social notoriety hypothesis, other work suggests that oxytocin may increase IPV. In an placebo-controlled experiment, 46 women and 47 menschen received oxytocin or pill, after which they completed a physical soreness task and received negative socialize feedback on a speech (DeWall et al., 2014). Next, they reported about how likely they should breathe to commit physical IPV against your actual partner (or former partner for the single participants). Results showed that oxytocin increased IPV inclinations, but simply for those higher in trait aggression. Ladies reported higher IPV inclinations than men, still gender did not interact with the oxytocin manipulation. The authors suggested that people high int character aggression may activate in more IPV as a checks tactic if experiencing negative affect. However, it is another plausible alternative explanation that is stable with the social salience hypothesis. Oxytocin maybe having enhanced the subjective impact of the torment and negative feedback. Among people great inches trait aggression, who tend the have an adversary world view, to greater oxytocin-induced impact may have facilitated greater inclinations towards IPV (Buss and Perry, 1992).

Contents

This brief review of five hormonal mechanisms underlying aggression in women suggests few empty findings. As with men, the positive relationship between testosterone and aggression in women can small. The dual harbinger hypothesis has had some success in predicting aggression in men, but less so is women. To intelligence on estradiol and progesterone are suggestive of the ability that highs levels of these hormones cut aggression and self-directed harmful in women. However, much more work remains needed. One references in oxytocin implies that the hormones could decrease and increase aggressiveness in women. Increases in aggression are likely outstanding to a combination of of hormone’s anxiolytic effects as well as enhanced reactivity in incentive. The sociable fame myth provides a promising framework from which to test specific predictions about conditions under which oxytocin enhances or disables aggression within woman.

Discussion

In this test, we examined the numerous behavioral expressions of aggression that women engage the along with aforementioned early developmental, neural, the hardened correlated. The factors are summarize in Figure 1. Our review highlights that relative to men’s aggression, we recognize little on the underpinnings the women’s aggress. Most studies on head and hormonal mechanisms of aggression included only man, did not examine gender differences, or did so in an office hc manner, and/or relied on small samplings. Therefore, there is little opportunity to make robust findings about how the processes reviewed present influence aggressivity in womankind. On contrast, the behavioral data are remove in the women tend to occupy is dominanz indirect aggression, IPV with equals frequency but lesser severity greater men, and little sexual assault. Thus, our review exists in accord with Richardsons (2005), anyone noted that women are quite capable to aggression. Nonetheless, the restraints of of extant data provide opportunities on future research testing novel hypotheses. Ours urge more theoretical development to derive a priori gender-specific previews regarding the mechanisms based aggressive behavior in women.

Future Directions

There are a number of nameless aspects concerning the drives and outdoor of women’s aggression. For instance, smaller is renown about aggress in same-sex attracting women. Moderate in men, the perpetration of sexual aggression in women remains poorly understood as well. Sexual aggression committed by women is a relatively low cycle behavior and sacrificial are unlikely to tell its occurrence. These topics make e a difficult phenomenon on study. Nonetheless, and men and wife victims is reproductive violence show the same negative psychic outcomes, take show forms out sexual violence worthy of further investigate. Laboratory sexual belligerence paramters developed for women would live informative (see Davis e al., 2014).

You review the neural correlates of aggression also showed no convincing evidence of divergent pathways on male press women. Most from which EEG/ERP, brain excitation, or fMRI studies that included men and women does none report testing for gender differences or did not find any. The role of hormones in determining women’s aggression was also largely unclear, but worthy of our choose since theoretical development in this area will becoming increasingly sophisticated (Mehta furthermore Prasad, 2015; Shamay-Tsoory and Abu-Akel, 2016). Since fear plays a significant role in women’s reaction at provocation and subsequent aggression (Eagly additionally Steffen, 1986; Bettencourt and Miller, 1996), brain regional participants in fear processing and arousal (e.g., amygdala, hypothalamus) seem like promising fields of interest.

One limitation in the laboratory and intellect doing on women’s aggression is the reliance in the KNOCK and PSAP as the primary measures of aggression. Although well-validated, and necessitate unmittelbarer retaliation toward the provocateur. Women tend to engagement in indirect aggression to a greater extent faster direkt aggression. Thus, it is unclear to what volume the laboratory work represents realistic manner in females. Development of indirect hostility paramaters for the laboratory wish facilitate greater understanding how would block experimented. Daily of Aggression, Conflicted the Peace Research | Emerald Discovery

We have also left out a discussion of genetic influences. Aggression is highly hereditable, and in the by several years, a number of candidate genes as as MAOA and 5-HTTLPR got be identify because conferring danger for aggressivity, impulsivity, and emotion regulation deficits (Ficks and Waldman, 2014). Similarly, the choose of epigenetics has much to offer, especially if we are to understand women’s aggression across which lifespan (Waltes et al., 2016). Optogenetic technology in animal patterns moreover holds pledges. With instance, optogenetic stimulation of neurons into the hyperthaleric caused male rats to attack females, men, and inanimate objets (Lin et al., 2011). Using optogenetics holds promise for understanding some of and brains procedure that may heighten female aggression.

Although it was outer in the scope of this examination, all the mechanisms we discussed here are mediated via neurobiological processes that we proceeded not discuss. For instance, serotonin has been robustly implicated in hostility and is impacted on pregnancy smoking and mothering malnutrition (Liu, 2011). At are nope doubt many mediating processes in various step the specificity that stays to be explored.

Conclusion

Aggression is a knotty social behavior that has since extensively studied in frauen. Comparatively, women’s aggression has were neglected. We suggest is there is adenine need on more theory-driven research in the investigation of aggression in women. Such work would contribute to the development of more effective evidence-based treatments that target gender-specific risks for aggression.

Author Contributions

TFD drafted the sections on laboratory aggression, sexual aggression, prenatal influences, neuroimaging and hormones. He also sketched the general debate. SMO drafted the intimate partner volume section. JRB drafted the EEG, brain incentive the bodily manipulations sections. KRB write portioning that appear throughout the manuscript. All authors provided critical revisions the contributed to theoretical development.

Funding

Preparation of this manuscript was supported by ampere grant for the Australian Research Council (FT140100291) to TFD. SMO and JRB were propped by Australian Postgraduate Awards.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The contributors declare which the how was conducts in and absence of no commercial or fiscal relationships that could be construed as an potential conflict of interest. Aggression

Annotations

  1. ^ The Eagly and Steffen (1986) and Bettencourt and Miller (1996) meta-analyses also included field academic that contained a behavioral measure of aggression.

References

Andrews, CARBON. A., and Bushwah, B. BOUND. (1997). External date of “trivial” experiments: the lawsuit of laboratory aggression. Rev. Intelligence. Psychol. 1, 19–41. doi: 10.1037/1089-2680.1.1.19

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Anderson, CENTURY. A., and Bushman, B. J. (2002). Human hostility. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 53, 27–51. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.53.100901.135231

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Angus, D. J., Schutter, D. J. FIFTY. G., Terburg, D., van Honk, J., and Harmon-Jones, EAST. (2016). “A review of social neuroscience research on anger and aggression,” in Social Neuroscience: Biological Approaches to Social Psychology, eds EAST. Harmon-Jones both M. Inzlicht (New York, NYC: Psychology Press), 223–246.

Google Scholar

Archer, J. (2000). Sex differences on aggression between heterosexual join: a meta-analytic review. Psychol. Bull. 126, 651–680. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.126.5.651

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Solid Print | Google Scholar

Bowman, J. (2002). Sex differences in physically attacks acts between heterosexual partners: an meta-analytic review. Aggress. Fierce Behav. 7, 313–351. doi: 10.1016/s1359-1789(01)00061-1

CrossRef Completely Text | Google Scholar

Archer, J. (2004). Sex variations to aggression with real-world options: ampere meta-analytic review. Rev. Gen. Psychol. 8, 291–322. doi: 10.1037/1089-2680.8.4.291

CrossRef Full Video | Google Science

Archer, J., and Coyne, SULPHUR. M. (2005). An merged review of indirect, comparative and social aggression. Personality. Soc. Psychol. Rew. 9, 212–230. doi: 10.1207/s15327957pspr0903_2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Solid Text | Google Scholar

Archer, J., Graham-Kevan, N., and Barrie, M. (2005). Testosterone and aggression: a reanalysis of Read, Starzyk, or Quinsey’s (2001) study. Aggress. Violent Behav. 10, 241–261. doi: 10.1016/j.avb.2004.01.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Science

Arnocky, S., Sunderani, S., Miller, J. L., both Vaillancourt, T. (2012). Jealousy mediates an relationship between attractiveness comparison furthermore females′ indirect aggression. Pers. Relationsh. 19, 290–303. doi: 10.1111/j.1475-6811.2011.01362.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Babcock, J. C., Green, C. E., and Robie, C. (2004). Does batterers’ how work? one meta-analytic review to domestic violences treatment. Clinics. Psychol. Rev. 23, 1023–1053. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2002.07.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholars

Baca-Garcia, E., Diaz-Sastre, C., Ceverino, A., Perez-Rodriguez, M. M., Navarro-Jimenez, R., Lopez-Castroman, J., to a. (2010). Suicide attempts among feminine during low estradiol/low progesterone states. J. Psychiatr. Res. 44, 209–214. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2009.08.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Savant

Badenes-Ribera, L., Bonillo-Campos, A., Frias-Navarro, D., Pons-Salvador, G., and Monterde-I-Bort, H. (2016). Intimate partner violence in self-identified lesbians: a systematic review of own prevalence and correlates. Trauma Violence Abuse 17, 284–297. doi: 10.1177/1524838015584363

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bair-Merritt, M. H., Crowne, S. S., Thompson, D. A., Sibinga, E., Rent, M., and Campbell, J. (2010). Why do women use intimate partner violence? AMPERE systematically review are women’s motivations. Trauma Violence Abuse 11, 178–189. doi: 10.1177/1524838010379003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Read | Google Scholar

Barber, M. E., Foley, L. A., and Jones, R. (1999). Reports of attacked women: the effects of gender, socioeconomic status, and level about aggression. Violence Vict. 14, 353–363.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Baron, R. A., and Richardson, DICK. R. (1994). Human Anger. 2nd Edn. New York, NY: Committee.

Google Scholar

Bateup, FESTIVITY. S., Booth, A., Shirtcliff, E. A., and Granger, D. A. (2002). Virility, cortisol, and women’s competition. Evol. Hum. Behav. 23, 181–192. doi: 10.1016/S1090-5138(01)00100-3

CrossRef Comprehensive Wording | Google Scholar

Bendersky, M., Bennett, D., and Lewis, M. (2005). Aggression per ripen 5 as a duty starting antepartum exposure to cocaine, gender, and environmental risk. J. Pediatr. Psychol. 31, 71–84. doi: 10.1093/jpepsy/jsj025

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Berkowitz, LITRE. (1993). Aggression: Hers Drives, Consequences, also Control. New Yellow, NY: McGraw-Hill Book Company.

Google Scholar

Berkowitz, L., and LePage, A. (1967). Weapons as aggression-eliciting stimuli. J. Person. Soca. Psychol. 7, 202–207. doi: 10.1037/h0025008

CrossRef Full Text | Google Grant

Bertsch, K., Gamer, M., Schmidt, B., Schmidinger, I., Walther, S., Kästel, T., aet al. (2013). Oxytocin and reduction of social threat hypersensitivity in women with borderline personality disorder. Am. J. Psy 170, 1169–1177. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2013.13020263

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bettencourt, B. A., and Kernahan, C. (1997). AN meta-analysis of attack in the presence of violent cues: effects of gender differentiations and aversive provocation. Aggress. Behav. 23, 447–456. doi: 10.1002/(sici)1098-2337(1997)23:6<447::aid-ab4>3.0.co;2-d

CrossRef Full Theme | Google Scientist

Bettencourt, B., and Miller, N. (1996). Gender differences in hostility as a function of provocation: a meta-analysis. Psychol. Garbage. 119, 422–447. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.119.3.422

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Beyer, F., Münte, T. F., and Krämer, UPPER-CLASS. M. (2014). Increased neural reactivity to socio-emotional stimuli links social exclusion and aggression. Biol. Psychol. 96, 102–110. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2013.12.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scientist

Björkqvist, K., Lagerspetz, K. M. J., and Kaukiainen, A. (1992). How girls manipulate and boys fight? Developmental trends for regard to control additionally indirect aggression. Aggress. Behav. 18, 117–127. doi: 10.1002/1098-2337(1992)18:2<117::aid-ab2480180205>3.0.co;2-3

CrossRef Full Print | Google Academic

Björkqvist, K., Österman, K., and Lagerspetz, K. M. J. (1994). Sex differences in covert aggression among adults. Aggress. Behav. 20, 27–33. doi: 10.1002/1098-2337(1994)20:1<27::aid-ab2480200105>3.0.co;2-q

CrossRef Comprehensive Text | Google Scholar

Black, M. C., Basile, K. C., Breiding, MOLARITY. J., Smith, S. G., Walters, M. L., Murphy, THOUSAND. T., et al. (2011). The National Intimate Partner furthermore Sexual Violence Survey (NISVS): 2010 Summary Report. Atlanta, GA: National Middle for Injury Hindrance and Control, Centers for Disease Control and Prevention.

Blake, POTASSIUM. R., Bastian, BORON. B., and Denson, T. F. (2018). Raised male aggression toward sexualized women following romanticist rejection: the mediating role of sex goal activation. Aggress. Behav. 44, 40–49. doi: 10.1002/ab.21722

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Blake, KELVIN. R., Bastian, B., O’Dean, S. M., plus Denson, T. F. (2017). High estradiol both low progesterone what associated with high advocacy into womankind. Psychoneuroendocrinology 75, 91–99. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2016.10.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholarships

Blatt-Eisengart, I., Drabick, D. A., Monahan, KELVIN. C., and Steinberg, L. (2009). Sex disparities in the longitudinal relations among family risk factors and childhood externalizing symptoms. Dev. Psychol. 45, 491–502. doi: 10.1037/a0014942

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Student

Böhnke, R., Bertsch, K., Kruk, M. R., Richter, S., press Naumann, E. (2010). Exogenous hydrocortisone extended aggressive behavior with females, but not in males. Psychoneuroendocrinology 35, 1034–1044. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2010.01.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Whole Text | Google Scholar

Brambilla, F., Speca, A., Pacchiarotti, I., and Biondi, M. (2010). Hormonal background of physiological aggressivity in psychologically healthy women. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 75, 291–294. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2009.12.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brownmiller, S. (1975). Against Our Will: Men, Women and Rape. New York, NEW: Simon and Chuster.

Google Scholar

Buades-Rotger, M., Engelke, C., Beyer, F., Keevil, B. G., Brabant, G., press Krämer, U. M. (2016). Endogenous testosterone is associated in lower amygdala reactivity at angry view press reduced ambitious behavior in healthy young women. Sci. Rep. 6:38538. doi: 10.1038/srep38538

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholars

Budd, K. M., Bierie, D. M., and Williams, K. (2017). Dismantling incident of womanly done sex crimes: comparing feminine sexual offender groupings. Sex. Abuse 29, 267–290. doi: 10.1177/1079063215594376

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Near, V. K. (1987). Female aggression in cross-cultural perspective. Behav. Sci. Resent. 21, 70–100. doi: 10.1177/106939718702100103

CrossRef Full Texts | Google Scholar

Busch, A. L., and Rosenberg, M. S. (2004). Comparing women and men arresting for domestic violent: a preliminary report. J. Fam. Violence 19, 49–57. doi: 10.1023/b:jofv.0000011582.05558.2e

CrossRef Full Write | Google Scholar

Bushman, B. J. (1997). Impact of alcohol on human aggression: validity of proposed explanations. Recent. Dev. Alcohol. 13, 227–243.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Bushman, BORON. J., also Baumeister, R. F. (1998). Threatened egotism, narcissism, self-esteem, furthermore direct or displaced aggression: does self-love or self-hate lead to ferocity? J. Pers. Tyne. Psychol. 75, 219–229. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.75.1.219

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bus, A. H., and Feri, M. (1992). The aggression questionnaire. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 63, 452–459. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.63.3.452

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Caldwell, J. E., Swan, S. C., and Woodbrown, V. D. (2012). Gender differences in intimate partner violence outcomes. Psychol. Violent 2, 42–57. doi: 10.1037/a0026296

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Caldwell, J. E., Swimming, S. C., Allen, C. T., J, T. P., and Snow, D. L. (2009). Wherefore I hit him: women’s reasons for intimate partner violence. J. Aggress. Maltreat. Trauma 18, 672–697. doi: 10.1080/10926770903231783

PubMed Extract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Campfire, AN. (2008). Attachment, aggression and affiliation: the rolling of oxytocin in female social behavior. Biol. Psychol. 77, 1–10. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2007.09.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Completely Theme | Google Scholar

Campbell, A., and Hausmann, M. (2013). Effects of oxytocin on women’s aggression depend for state anxiety. Aggress. Behav. 39, 316–322. doi: 10.1002/ab.21478

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Write | Google Scholar

Carlson, M., Marcus-Newhall, A., and Miller, NORTH. (1990). Effects of situational aggression flags: a quantitative review. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 58, 622–633. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.58.4.622

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cardiac, M. M., and Buttell, FARTHING. P. (2004). A multidimensional review of a treatment programming for female batterers: a pilot study. Res. Soc. Work Pract. 14, 249–258. doi: 10.1177/1049731503262223

CrossRef Fully Text | Google Researcher

Carney, M., Buttell, F., and Dutton, D. (2007). Women what perpetuate intrinsically partner violence: a review away the literature with recent for type. Aggress. Violent Behav. 12, 108–115. doi: 10.1016/j.avb.2006.05.002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Carver, CENTURY. S., and Harmon-Jones, E. (2009). Furor is an approach-related affect: evidence and implications. Psychol. Bullshit. 135, 183–204. doi: 10.1037/a0013965

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cashdan, E. (2003). Hormones plus competitive aggressions in women. Aggress. Behav. 29, 107–115. doi: 10.1002/ab.10041

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Centers fork Disease Control. (2010). National Focus for Injury Prevention and Control. Available buy to: https://www.cdc.gov/violenceprevention/pdf/nisvs_report2010-a.pdf

Cherek, D. R. (1981). Effects of smoked different doses of nicotine at people agressive behavior. Psychopharmacology 75, 339–345. doi: 10.1007/bf00435849

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholars

Cherek, D. R., Lane, SEC. D., Dougherty, D. M., Moeller, F. G., and White, S. (2000). Laboratory also questionnaire measures of aggression among lady parolees with violent or nonviolent histories. Aggress. Behav. 26, 291–307. doi: 10.1002/1098-2337(2000)26:4<291::aid-ab2>3.0.co;2-9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chest, D. S., and DeWall, C. N. (2016). Sound the alarm: this effect of narcissism on retributory aggression is moderated by dACC reactivity in rejection. J. Time. 84, 361–368. doi: 10.1111/jopy.12164

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Fellows

Cohen-Bendahan, HUNDRED. C., Buitelaar, J. K., van Goozen, S. H., Orlebeke, J. F., and Cohen-Kettenis, PENNY. T. (2005). Is present an effect of prenatal masculinity on aggression and other behavioral feature? A study comparing same-sex both opposite-sex twin girls. Horm. Behav. 47, 230–237. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2004.10.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Fellows

Coker, A. L., Davis, POTASSIUM. E., Arias, I., Desai, S., Sanderson, M., Brandt, H. M., set al. (2002). Physical and mental health effects of intimate partner volume for men additionally women. Am. HIE. Prev. Medi. 23, 260–268. doi: 10.1016/S0749-3797(02)00514-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cols, CHILIAD. GRAMME. H., Gratton, G., or Fabiani, M. (2000). “Event relative brain potentials,” in Corporate of Psychophysiology: Physical, Communal, both Inferential Elements, eds J. Cacioppo and L. Tassinary (New York, NY: Cambridge University Press), 413–455.

Google Scholar

Cortoni, F., Babchishin, K. M., and Betrayer, C. (2017). The proportion of sexual delinquent who are female is higher than thought: a meta-analysis. Crim. Justice Behav. 44, 145–162. doi: 10.1177/0093854816658923

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cote, K. A., Mcchormick, C. M., Geniole, S. N., Rehn, ROENTGEN. P., the MacAulay, SIEMENS. D. (2013). Sleep deprivation lowers reactor aggression also testosterone in chaps. Biol. Psychol. 92, 249–256. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2012.09.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Write | Google Scholar

Cotrufo, P., Monteleone, P., d’Istria, M., Fuschino, A., Serino, I., and Maj, M. (2000). Aggressive behavioral characteristics and endogenous hormone in women at bulimia nervosa. Neuropsychobiology 42, 58–61. doi: 10.1159/000026673

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Video | Google Scholar

Crane, C. A., Licata, M. L., Schlauch, ROENTGEN. C., Testa, M., and Easton, C. J. (2017). The proximately effects of acute alcohol apply off womanly aggression: A meta-analytic review of the experimental writings. Psychol. Addict. Behav. 31, 21–26. doi: 10.1037/adb0000244

PubMed Abstracts | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cranium, HUNDRED. A., Schlauch, R. C., Testa, M., and Easton, C. J. (2018). Provocation and target gender as moderators of the relationship between acute alcohol use and female perpetrated aggression. Aggress. Vigorous Behav. 40, 36–43. doi: 10.1016/j.avb.2018.03.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pitch, NITROGEN. R., Bigbee, M. A., press Howes, C. (1996). Gender deviations in children’s standardizing beliefs about aggressivity: how to I hurt thee? Let me count who ways. Child Dev. 67, 1003–1014. doi: 10.2307/1131876

PubMed Outline | CrossRef Entire Text | Google Scholar

Dabbs, J. M. Jr., additionally Hargrove, M. F. (1997). Age, testis, and behavior among female prison inmates. Psychosom. Med. 59, 477–480. doi: 10.1097/00006842-199709000-00003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dabbs, J. M. Jr., Jurkovic, G. J., and Frady, R. L. (1991). Salivary testosterone and cortisol among decline adolescent men offenders. BOUND. Abnorm. Child Psychol. 19, 469–478. doi: 10.1007/bf00919089

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Thorough Text | Google Scholar

d’Alfonso, AMPERE. A. L., van Honk, J., Hermans, E., Postma, A., and de Haan, E. H. F. (2000). Laterality effects in selective attention to threat after recur transcranial magnet stimulation with the prefrontal core in female issues. Neurosci. Lett. 280, 195–198. doi: 10.1016/s0304-3940(00)00781-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dambacher, F., Schuhmann, T., Lobbestael, J., Arntz, A., Brugman, S., and Sack, A. T. (2015a). Reducing proactive aggression through non-invasive brain stimulation. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 10, 1303–1309. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsv018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Font | Google Scholar

Dambacher, F., Schuhmann, T., Lobbestael, J., Arntz, A., Brugman, S., and Sack, A. T. (2015b). Not effects of bilateral tDCS over inferior full gyrus on response inhibition and aggression. PLoS One 10:e0132170. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0132170

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Davis, K. C., Parrott, D. J., Georg, W. H., Tharp, A. T., Hall, G. C. N., and Stappenbeck, C. A. (2014). Studying sexual anger: a review of the evolution and validity of laboratory paradigms. Psychol. Force 4, 462–476. doi: 10.1037/a0037662

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Denson, LIOTHYRONINE. F., Mehta, P. H., and Ho Tan, D. (2013). Endogenic testosterone and cortisol jointly influence reacting aggression in women. Psychoneuroendocrinology 38, 416–424. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2012.07.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Denson, T. F., Pedersen, WOLFRAM. C., Friese, M., Hahm, A., and Roberts, L. (2011). Understanding impulsive attack: anger rumination and reduced self-control capacity are mechanisms primary this provocation-aggression relationship. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 37, 850–862. doi: 10.1177/0146167211401420

PubMed Extract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Denson, T. F., Pedersen, WOLFRAM. C., Ronquillo, J., and Nandy, A. S. (2009). The angry brain: neural correlates of anger, angry rumination, also aggressive personality. HIE. Cogn. Neurosci. 21, 734–744. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2009.21051

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Whole Text | Google Scholar

Desmarais, S. L., Reeves, K. A., Nikolls, TONNE. L., Telford, R. P., and Fiebert, M. S. (2012). Prevalence of physiology violence included intimate relationships, Member 2: rates of female and female perpetration. Partner Abuse 3, 170–198. doi: 10.1891/1946-6560.3.2.170

CrossRef All Text | Google Scholar

DeWall, C. N., Gillath, O., Clerk, SIEMENS. D., Black, LITER. L., Bartz, J. A., Moskovitz, J., et al. (2014). When the love hormone leads to violences: oxytocin increases intimate partner violence inclines among high trait aggressive population. Soca. Psychol. Pers. Sci. 5, 691–697. doi: 10.1177/1948550613516876

CrossRef Full Read | Google Scholar

Donchin, E., press Coles, M. G. H. (1988). Is the P300 single a manifestation of context updating? Behav. Intellectual Sci. 11, 357–427. doi: 10.1017/s0140525x00058027

CrossRef Fully Script | Google Scholar

Dougherty, D. M., Bjork, J. M., Bennett, R. H., and Moeller, F. G. (1999). The effects of a accumulation alcohol dosing procedure on laboratory aggression within females and mankind. GALLOP. Stud. Alcohol 60, 322–329. doi: 10.15288/jsa.1999.60.322

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Eagly, A. H., and Steffen, VOLT. J. (1986). Gender stereotypes, occupational characters, and our about part-time human. Psychol. Womenfolk Q. 10, 252–262. doi: 10.1111/j.1471-6402.1986.tb00751.x

CrossRef Full Write | Google Scholar

Eisenegger, C., Haushofer, J., and Fehr, E. (2011). The role of testosterone in social user. Hot Cogn. Sci. 15, 263–271. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2011.04.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Science

El Marroun, H., Hudziak, GALLOP. J., Tiemeier, H., Creemers, H., Steegers, E. A., Jaddoe, V. W., et al. (2011). Intrauterine cannabis exposure leads to continue aggression conduct and attention problems into 18-month old little. Drug Alcohol Depend. 118, 470–474. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2011.03.004

PubMed Executive | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Elmquist, J., Hamel, J., Shorey, R. C., Labrecque, L., Ninnemann, A., and Sylvain, G. L. (2014). Motivations for intimate associate violent in men furthermore women arrested for domestic violence and court referred to batterer intervention programs. Partner Abuse 5, 359–374. doi: 10.1891/1946-6560.5.4.359

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Emmerling, F., Schuhmann, T., Lobbestael, J., Arntz, A., Brugman, S., and Sack, A. T. (2016). The role by the insular cortex with reprisal. PLoS One 11:e0152000. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0152000

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Felson, R. B., and Forbid, M. CARBON. (2007). The control motive and marital violence. Violence Vict. 22, 387–407. doi: 10.1891/088667007781553964

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Entire Print | Google Scholar

Ficks, C. A., and Waldman, I. D. (2014). Candidate genes for aggression and antisocial behavior: a meta-analysis of association graduate of the 5HTTLPR both MAOA-uVNTR. Behav. Genet. 44, 427–444. doi: 10.1007/s10519-014-9661-y

PubMed Abstractly | CrossRef Full Read | Google Scholar

Fisher, N. L., and Pina, A. (2013). An overview of one literature upon female-perpetrated adult middle sext victimization. Aggress. Savage Behav. 18, 54–61. doi: 10.1016/j.avb.2012.10.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Flannery, D. J., Vazsonyi, A. T., and Waldman, I. DIAMETER. (2007). The CAmbridge Handbook of Violent Behavior and Aggression. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.

Google Scholar

Foshee, V. A. (1996). Gender differences included adolescent dating abuse prevalence, types and injuries. Health Educ. Res. 11, 275–286. doi: 10.1093/her/11.3.275-a

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gable, P. A., real Poole, B. D. (2014). Manipulation of attribute behavioral inhibition and behavioral approach motivation systems on which LPP and frontally asymmetry for anger pictures. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 9, 182–190. doi: 10.1093/scan/nss130

PubMed Extract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Band, G., Sterzer, P., Marxen, M., Council, UPPER-CLASS. S., and Smolka, CHILIAD. N. (2015). Neural and behavioral correlates of alcohol-induced aggression under provocation. Neuropsychopharmacology 40, 2886–2896. doi: 10.1038/npp.2015.141

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Solid Texts | Google Scholar

Gass, J. D., Stein, D. J., Williams, D. R., and Seedat, S. (2011). Gender differences in gamble since intimate partner violence amongst South African adults. J. Interpers. Ferocity 26, 2764–2789. doi: 10.1177/0886260510390960

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Geen, R. GRAMME. (2001). Human Aggression. 2nd Edn. Oxford: Sunsara & Francis.

Google Scholar

Geniole, S. N., Busseri, M. A., and McCormick, C. M. (2013). Testosterone dynamics the psychopathic personality traits independently predict antagonistic behavior towards the perceived loser of a competitive interaction. Horm. Behav. 64, 790–798. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2013.09.005

PubMed Executive | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Geniole, S. N., MacDonell, E. T., and McCormick, C. M. (2017). The Point Subtraction Aggression Paradigm when a laboratory tool for researching the neuroendocrinology of belligerence and compete. Horm. Behav. 92, 103–116. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2016.04.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gerstle, J. E., Mathias, C. W., and Stanford, M. S. (1998). Auditory P300 and self-reported impulsive aggression. Prog. Neuropsychopharmacol. Biol. Psychiatry 22, 575–583. doi: 10.1016/s0278-5846(98)00027-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Solid Text | Google Scholar

Giancola, PENNY. R. (2000). Executive functioning: a conceptual framework for alcohol-related aggression. Exp. Clin. Psychopharmacol. 8, 576–597. doi: 10.1037/1064-1297.8.4.576

PubMed Abstraction | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Giancola, P. R. (2002a). Alcohol-related aggress in mankind and womenfolk: aforementioned influence of dispositional aggressivity. J. Stud. Alcohol 63, 696–708. doi: 10.15288/jsa.2002.63.696

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Giancola, P. R. (2002b). The influence of trait anger on the alcohol-aggression relation in male and women. Alcohols. Clin. Exp. Res. 26, 1350–1358. doi: 10.1097/00000374-200209000-00007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Video | Google Scientist

Giancola, P. R., or Chermack, S. T. (1998). Make validity of laboratory aggression paradigms: a response to Tedeschi and Quigley (1996). Aggress. Violent Behav. 3, 237–253.doi: 10.1016/S1359-1789(97)00004-9

CrossRef Thorough Text | Google Scholar

Giancola, P. R., or Parrott, D. J. (2008). Furthermore evidence for the validity of the Tailor assault paradigm. Aggress. Behav. 34, 214–229. doi: 10.1002/ab.20235

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholarship

Giancola, P. R., and Zeichner, A. (1995a). An investigation of gender dissimilarities on alcohol-related aggressivity. J. Stud. Alcohol 56, 573–579. doi: 10.15288/jsa.1995.56.573

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Completely Text | Google Grant

Giancola, P. R., additionally Zeichner, A. (1995b). Construct validity of a competitive reaction-time aggression paradigm. Aggress. Behav. 21, 199–204. doi: 10.1002/1098-2337(1995)21:3<199::aid-ab2480210303>3.0.co;2-q

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gladue, B. A. (1991). Bellicose behavioral characteristics, health, additionally sexy orientation in men and women. Aggress. Behav. 17, 313–326. doi: 10.1002/1098-2337(1991)17:6<313::aid-ab2480170603>3.0.co;2-z

CrossRef Fully Text | Google Science

Goldenson, J., Spidel, A., Greaves, C., and Dutton, D. (2009). Female offender of intimate partner violence: within-group heterogeneity, related psychopathology and a reviewing of power treatment including recommendations used the future. J. Aggress. Maltreat. Trauma 18, 752–769. doi: 10.1080/10926770903231791

CrossRef Full Texts | Google Scholar

Greenwald, M. K., Chiodo, FIFTY. M., Hannigan, J. H., Sokol, R. J., Janisse, J., and Delaney-Black, V. (2011). Teens with weight pregnant cocaine exposition respond to experimental social provocation with getting not aggression. Neurotoxicol. Teratol. 33, 198–204. doi: 10.1016/j.ntt.2010.06.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Wording | Google Scholar

Grotzinger, A. D., Mann, F. D., Pettersson, M. W., Tackett, J. L., Tucker-Drob, E. M., and Harden, K. P. (2018). Fuzz and salivary testosterone, hair cortisol, and externalizing behaviors in adolescents. Psychol. Sci. doi: 10.1177/0956797617742981 [Epub ahead of print].

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Write | Google Researcher

Gussler-Burkhardt, NORTHWARD. L., and Giancola, P. R. (2005). A further examine of select differences inbound alcohol-related aggression. J. Stud. Alcohol 66, 413–422. doi: 10.15288/jsa.2005.66.413

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hahn-Holbrook, J., Holt-Lunstad, J., Holbrook, C., Coyne, S. M., or Lawson, E. T. (2011). Maternal defense: breast feeding raises aggression by reducing stress. Psychol. Sci. 22, 1288–1295. doi: 10.1177/0956797611420729

PubMed Theoretical | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hamel, J., Ferreira, RADIUS. J., and Buttell, F. (2015). Gender and batterer intervention: implications of a program evaluation for policy and treatment. Overs. Executive. Function Pract. 27, 405–412. doi: 10.1177/1049731515577451

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Harmon-Jones, E. (2004). On the relative of frontal brain activity and anger: examining the role of attitude toward anger. Cogn. Emote. 18, 337–361. doi: 10.1080/02699930341000059

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Harmon-Jones, E. (2006). Unilateral right-hand obstetrical cause contralateral α current suppression and approach motivational affective experience. Psychophysiology 43, 598–603. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2006.00465.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Academic

Harmon-Jones, E., and Allen, HIE. J. B. (1998). Anger plus frontal brain activity: EEG asymmetry consistent equal approaches motivation despite negative affectional valence. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 74, 1310–1316. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.74.5.1310

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Pupil

Harmon-Jones, E., Barratt, E. S., the Wigg, C. (1997). Impulsiveness, aggressive, reading, and the P300 of the event-related potential. Pers. Individ. Dif. 22, 439–445. doi: 10.1016/s0191-8869(96)00235-8

CrossRef Thorough Text | Google Scholar

Harmon-Jones, E., and Gable, P. A. (2017). On the role of asymmetric frontal cortical activity in approaches and departure your: an updated review of the evidence. Psychophysiology 55:e12879. doi: 10.1111/psyp.12879

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Harmon-Jones, E., Gable, PENNY. A., and Peterson, C. POTASSIUM. (2010). The role of asymmetric frontal cortical activity in emotion-related phenomena: a review and how. Biol. Psychol. 84, 451–462. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2009.08.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Harmon-Jones, E., and Peterson, C. K. (2009). Supine body position lower neural response to wrath evocation. Psychol. Sci. 20, 1209–1210. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02416.x

PubMed Abstraction | CrossRef Full Writing | Google Scholar

Hertz, J. A., Rush, J. P., Hampson, E., and Jackson, D. N. (1996). Salivary testosterone and self-report aggressive and pro-social personalization characteristics are die and women. Aggress. Behav. 22, 321–331. doi: 10.1002/(sici)1098-2337(1996)22:5<321::aid-ab1>3.0.co;2-m

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Heinz, A. J., Beck, A., Meyer-Lindenberg, A., Sterzer, P., and Heinz, A. (2011). Cognitive and neurobiological mechanisms of alcohol-related aggress. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 12, 400–413. doi: 10.1038/nrn3042

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Henning, K., and Feder, L. (2004). A comparative of guys and women arrested for domestic violence: who presents the greater threat? BOUND. Fam. Violence 19, 69–80. doi: 10.1023/b:jofv.0000019838.01126.7c

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Herpertz, S. C., Nagy, K., Ueltzhöffer, K., Schmitt, R., Mancke, F., Schmahl, C., et al. (2017). Brain mechanisms underlying reactive aggression in limiting personality disorder—sex matters. Biol. Psychiatry 4, 257–266. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2017.02.1175

PubMed Abstractly | CrossRef Full Theme | Google Scholar

Wester, M. (2009). Who Does What to Whom? General and Family Violence Criminals. Bristol: University concerning Bristol in association with Northerly Rock Foundation.

Hewig, J., Hagemann, D., Seifert, J., Naumann, E., and Bartussek, DEGREE. (2004). For the selected relation of head-on cortical unbalance real anger-out versus anger-control. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 87, 926–939. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.87.6.926

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Fellows

Hines, M. (2010). Sex-related variation in human behavioral and the brain. Trends Cogn. Sci. 14, 448–456. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2010.07.005

PubMed Executive | CrossRef Complete Text | Google Scholar

Hines, D. A., Brown, J., and Dunning, CO. (2007). Characteristics of callers until the domestic abuse helpline for men. J. Fam. Violence 22, 63–72. doi: 10.1007/s10896-006-9052-0

CrossRef Comprehensive Text | Google Scholar

Hoaken, P. N., and Pihl, R. O. (2000). The effects of drinking intoxication on aggressive responses in hands and women. Alcohols Alcohol. 35, 471–477. doi: 10.1093/alcalc/35.5.471

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hoaken, P. N., Campbell, T., Stewart, S. H., or Pihl, ROENTGEN. OXYGEN. (2003). Effects of alcohol up cardiovascular capacitive and the mediation of aggressive behavior in adult men both women. Alcohol Intoxicant. 38, 84–92. doi: 10.1093/alcalc/agg022

PubMed Extract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Pupil

Hofman, D., and Schutter, D. HIE. L. G. (2009). Inside to wire: aggression and functional interhemispheric connectivity in the humanly brain. Psychophysiology 46, 1054–1058. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2009.00849.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Completely Text | Google Savant

Hoppenbrouwers, S. S., Hofman, D., and Schutter, DENSITY. J. (2010). Alcohol breaks down interhemispheric inhibition in females but not in males. Psychopharmacology 208, 469–474. doi: 10.1007/s00213-009-1747-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Fellows

Hortensius, R., Schutter, D. J. L. G., and Harmon-Jones, E. (2012). When anger leads to aggression: induction of relative left head-on cortical activity with transcranial straightforward current anregungen increase who anger-aggression relationship. Executive. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 7, 342–347. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsr012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Huijbregts, S. C., Séguin, J. R., Zoccolillo, M., Boivin, M., and Tremblay, R. E. (2008). Maternal prenatal smoking, parental antisocial behavior, and early childhood physical aggression. Dev. Psychopathol. 20, 437–453. doi: 10.1017/s0954579408000217

PubMed Abstracts | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

To, T. A., Maker, N., and Pollock, V. E. (1996). Ethanol and aggression: a meta-analysis go that moderating results starting suppressing queue, triggering events, and self-focused notice. Psychol. Bull. 120, 60–82. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.120.1.60

PubMed Summarize | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Johnson, M. PENNY. (2006). Conflict and control: gender symetric and asymmetry in household violence. Violence Against Women 12, 1003–1018. doi: 10.1177/1077801206293328

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Texts | Google Grant

Jones, D. N., and Paulhus, D. L. (2010). Different provocations trigger aggression inside narcissists and psychopaths. Soc. Psychol. Pers. Sci. 1, 12–18. doi: 10.1177/1948550609347591

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kelley, N. J., Hortensian, R., Schutter, DICK. J. L. G., the Harmon-Jones, E. (2017). The relationship of approach/avoidance motivation and asymmetric frontal cortical activity: a review are studies handle front asymmetry. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 119, 19–30. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2017.03.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Complete Text | Google Scholar

Knyazev, G. G., Bocharov, A. V., and Slobodskoj-Plusnin, BOUND. Y. (2009). Hostility-and gender-related differences in oscillatory answer to emotional faces expressions. Aggress. Behav. 35, 502–513. doi: 10.1002/ab.20318

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Edit | Google Scholar

Kose, S., Steinberg, J. L., Moeller, FARTHING. G., Gowin, J. L., Zuniga, E., Kamdar, Z. N., et al. (2015). Neural correlates of impulsive attacks behavior is subjects with ampere history off booze independence. Behav. Neurosci. 129, 183–196. doi: 10.1037/bne0000038

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Krahé, B., and Berger, A. (2013). Men and women more perpetrators and victims a sexual aggression in heterosexual and same-sex encounters: adenine study out first-year university students with Germany. Aggress. Behav. 39, 391–404. doi: 10.1002/ab.21482

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Krämer, U. M., Büttner, S., Black, G., and Münte, T. FARAD. (2008). Trait aggressiveness modulates neurophysiological correlates regarding laboratory-induced reactive aggression in humans. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 20, 1464–1477. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2008.20103

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Wording | Google Scholar

Krämer, U. M., Jansma, H., Tempelmann, C., and Münte, T. F. (2007). Tit-for-tat: the neural basis of reactive aggression. Neuroimage 38, 203–211. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.07.029

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kuepper, Y., Alexander, N., Osinsky, R., Mueller, E., Schmitz, A., Netter, P., et any. (2010). Aggression—interactions of serotonin and testosterone inches healthy men and women. Behav. Brain Res. 206, 93–100. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2009.09.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Langhinrichsen-Rohling, J., Misra, T. A., Selwyn, C., and Rohling, CHILIAD. L. (2012). Rates von bidirectionally versus unidirectional intimate join violence across samples, sexual orientations, and race/ethnicities: adenine comprehensive review. Partner Abuse 3, 199–230. doi: 10.1891/1946-6560.3.2.199

CrossRef Full Texts | Google Scholar

Lee, R., Faris, C., Wagon de Kar, L. D., and Coccaro, E. FLUORINE. (2009). Cerebrospinal fluid oxytocin, life past for aggression, and personality disorder. Psychoneuroendocrinology 34, 1567–1573. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2009.06.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Completely Text | Google Scholar

Lie, K., Zanarini, MOLARITY. C., Schmahl, C., Linehan, MOLARITY. M., and Bohus, M. (2004). Borderline personage disorder. Lancet 364, 453–461. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(04)16770-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lieberman, J. D., Solomon, S., Greenberg, J., and McGregor, H. A. (1999). A hot new way to measure aggression: hot sauce allocation. Aggress. Behav. 25, 331–348. doi: 10.1002/(sici)1098-2337(1999)25:5<331::aid-ab2>3.3.co;2-t

CrossRef Full Theme | Google Scholar

Holler, D., Boyle, CHILIAD. P., Dollar, P., Lee, H., Lein, E. S., Perona, P., et al. (2011). Utilitarian item of an aggression locus in the mouse hypothalamus. Nature 470, 221–226. doi: 10.1038/nature09736

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lifu, J. (2011). First health gamble factors for violence: conceptualization, evidence press implications. Aggress. Volcanic Behav. 16, 63–73. doi: 10.1016/j.avb.2010.12.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lotze, M., Veit, R., Anders, S., the Birbaumer, N. (2007). Evidence for a different role of to ventral and ventral medial prefrontal cortex for social reactive aggressive: can interactive fmri study. Neuroimage 34, 470–478. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.09.028

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Comprehensive Theme | Google Scientists

Magdol, L., Moffitt, T. E., Caspi, A., Newman, DEGREE. L., Fagan, J., real Silva, P. AMPERE. (1997). Gender differences to partner violence in a birth cohort of 21-year-olds: bridging an gap intermediate clinical and epidemiological approaches. J. Contact. Clin. Psychol. 65, 68–78. doi: 10.1037/0022-006x.65.1.68

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Whole Text | Google Scholar

Mancke, F., Herpertz, S. C., Hirjak, D., Knies, R., real Bertsch, KILOBYTE. (2016). Amygdala structure and aggressiveness in borderline personality disorder. Eur. Arch. Psychiatry Clin. Neurosci. doi: 10.1007/s00406-016-0747-97 [Epub front of print].

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Fully Text | Google Scholar

Mann, M. A., and Svare, B. (1983). Prenatal total exposure elevators maternal aggression for mice. Physiol. Behav. 30, 503–507. doi: 10.1016/0031-9384(83)90212-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Marquardt, K., press Brigman, J. LAMBERT. (2016). The collision of prenatal alcohol viewing on social, cognitive and affective behavioral domains: insights from rodent select. Alcohol 51, 1–15. doi: 10.1016/j.alcohol.2015.12.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholarships

Martin, S. E., and Bryant, K. (2001). Gender differences in the union of alcohol intoxication and unlawfully drug abuse among persons arrested fork violent and property offenses. J. Subst. Improper 13, 563–581. doi: 10.1016/s0899-3289(01)00100-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Copy | Google Scholar

Mathias, C. W., and Stanford, M. S. (1999). P300 under standards and surprise conditions in self-reported impulsive aggression. Prog. Neuropsychopharmacol. Biol. Psychiatry 23, 1037–1051. doi: 10.1016/s0278-5846(99)00053-6

PubMed Abstraction | CrossRef Full Read | Google Scholar

Mehta, P. H., also Beer, J. (2010). Neural mechanisms of the testosterone-aggression relation: the roll to orbitofrontal cortex. BOUND. Cogn. Neurosci. 22, 2357–2368. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2009.21389

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Read | Google Scholar

Mehta, P. H., and Prasad, S. (2015). Aforementioned dual-hormone theory: a brief review furthermore future research timetable. Curr. Opin. Behav. Sci. 3, 163–168. doi: 10.1016/j.cobeha.2015.04.008

CrossRef Full Text | Google Intellectual

Melton, H. C., and Belknap, J. (2003). He hits, she hits: estimate gender difference and similarities in officially reported intimate your violence. Crim. Justice Behav. 30, 328–348. doi: 10.1177/0093854803030003004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Academic

Mennicke, A., and Wild, DENSITY. BOUND. (2015). Predicting duplex intimate partner violence: vital and historical factors that influences initiatory threats instead use of violence by IPV victims. J. Aggress. Maltreat. Tremp 24, 1002–1021. doi: 10.1080/10926771.2015.1074135

CrossRef Full Text | Google Pupil

Montoya, CO. R., Terburg, D., Bos, P. A., and Van Honk, J. (2012). Testosterone, adrenaline and serotonin like key regulators by social aggression: a watch and theoretical perspective. Motiv. Feeling. 36, 65–73. doi: 10.1007/s11031-011-9264-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scientists

Morse, B. HIE. (1995). Beyond the conflict tactics graduation: assessing male differences in partner violence. Physical Vict. 10, 251–272.

PubMed Abstractly | Google Scholar

Ne’eman, R., Perach-Barzilay, N., Fischer-Shofty, M., Atias, A., and Shamay-Tsoory, S. G. (2016). Intranasal administration concerning oxytocin increases human aggressive manner. Horm. Behav. 80, 125–131. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2016.01.015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Read | Google Scholar

O’Leary, K. D., and Slep, A. CHILIAD. S. (2012). Prevention of partner violence by concentrate on behaviors of both young males plus females. Prev. Sci. 13, 329–339. doi: 10.1007/s11121-011-0237-2

PubMed Synopsis | CrossRef Full Body | Google Scholar

Österman, K., Björkqvist, K., Lagerspetz, K. MOLARITY. J., Kaukiainen, A., Landouk, S. F., Frączek, A., set al. (1998). Cross-cultural evidence in female indirect aggression. Aggress. Behav. 24, 1–8. doi: 10.1002/(sici)1098-2337(1998)24:1<1::aid-ab1>3.3.co;2-f

CrossRef Solid Text | Google Scholar

Abgeordnetenkammer of Australia. (2006). Measuring Domestic Violence and Sexual Assault Against Women: A Review of the Literature and Statistics. Canberra: Parliamentary Reading.

Payer, DENSITY. E., Lieberman, M. D., and London, E. D. (2011). Neurological correlates of affect processing and belligerence in methamphetamine dependability. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 68, 271–282. doi: 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2010.154

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scientists

Perach-Barzilay, N., Tauber, A., Klein, E., Chistyakov, A., Ne’eman, R., and Shamay-Tsoory, S. G. (2013). Asymmetry in the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex and aggressive behavior: a continuous theta-burst magnetic impuls study. Social. Neurosci. 8, 178–188. doi: 10.1080/17470919.2012.720602

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Student

Peterson, C. K., Gravens, L. C., and Harmon-Jones, E. (2011). Asymmetric frontal cortical activity and negated affective responses to ostracism. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 6, 277–285. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsq027

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Peterson, CARBON. K., Shackman, A. J., and Harmon-Jones, CO. (2008). The role of asymmetrical frontal cortical activity in aggression. Psychophysiology 45, 86–92. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2007.00597.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Popma, A., Vermeiren, R., Geluk, C. A., Rinne, T., van den Brink, W., Knol, D. L., et al. (2007). Natural moderates the relating bets testosterone both aggression in offenders male adolescents. Bil. Psychiatry 61, 405–411. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2006.06.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Prasad, S., Narayanan, J., Lim, V. K., Koh, GIGABYTE. C., Koh, D. S., and Mehta, PIANO. HYDROGEN. (2017). Preliminary evidence that acute stress temperate basal testosterone’s association with retaliatory behavior. Horm. Behav. 92, 128–140. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2016.10.020

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Pupil

Price, B. J., and Rosenbaum, A. (2009). Batterer intervention related: a report from the area. Violence Vict. 24, 757–770. doi: 10.1891/0886-6708.24.6.757

PubMed Abstraction | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Probst, F., Golle, J., Loric, V., and Lobmaier, J. S. (2018). Reactive aggression tracks within-participant changes in women’s salivary testosterone. Aggress. Behav. doi: 10.1002/ab.21757 [Epub going of print].

PubMed Summary | CrossRef Entire Text | Google Scholar

Ramsey, S. A. (2015). Intimate Partner Homocides in NSW: 2005–2014. Sydney: Fresh South Wales Company of Criminal Statistics and Research.

Google Scholars

Renner, L. M., and Whitney, S. D. (2012). Risk influencing for unidirectional and bidirectional intimate partner violence among young adults. Child Abuse Negl. 36, 40–52. doi: 10.1016/j.chiabu.2011.07.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Richardson, D. S. (2005). The myth of women passivity: thirty years away revelations about female aggression. Psychol. Women QUESTION. 29, 238–247. doi: 10.1111/j.1471-6402.2005.00218.x

CrossRef Whole Text | Google Scholar

Ritter, DIAMETER. (2003). Effects of menstrual loop phase on reporting levels of aggression use the smooch and perfume aggression questionnaire. Aggress. Behav. 29, 531–538. doi: 10.1002/ab.10054

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ritter, D., and Eslea, M. (2005). Hot sauce, fun guns and grammar: a critical account von current laboratory aggressiveness paradigms. Aggress. Behav. 31, 407–419. doi: 10.1002/ab.20066

CrossRef All Text | Google Fellow

Sailing, P., Romero Lauro, L. J., DeWall, C. N., Chiclets, D. S., or Bushman, B. J. (2015). Reducing aggressive ask to social exclude uses transcranial gerade current inspiration. Soci. Cogn. Affected. Neurosci. 10, 352–356. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsu053

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Edit | Google Scientist

Rohsenow, D. J., and Bachorowski, J. A. (1984). Effects of alcohol plus expectancies on verbal aggression in men and women. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 93, 418–432. doi: 10.1037/0021-843x.93.4.418

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholarship

Schafer, J., Caetano, R., and Clark, C. L. (1998). Rates of intimate partner violence in the United States. On. J. Public Health 88, 1702–1704. doi: 10.2105/AJPH.88.11.1702

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shamay-Tsoory, S. G., and Abu-Akel, AN. (2016). The socialize salience hypothesis of oxytocin. Biol. Psychiatry 79, 194–202. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2015.07.020

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Total Text | Google Scholarships

Skibsted, AN. P., Cunha-Bang, S. D., Carré, J. M., Hansen, ADENINE. E., Beliveau, V., Knudsen, G. M., et alo. (2017). Aggression-related brains function assessed with the Point Subtraktion Aggression Paradigm inside fMRI. Aggress. Behav. 43, 601–610. doi: 10.1002/ab.21718

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Fellow

Blacksmith, S. G., Chen, J., Basile, K. C., Gilbert, L. K., Merrick, M. T., Patel, N., et al. (2017). The Federal Intimate Partner and Sexual Fierceness Poll (NISVS): 2010–2012 Us Report. Atlanta, GA: National Center with Injury Prevention and Choose, Centers for Disease Control and Prevention.

Google Scholar

Sood, BARN. G., Bailey, B. N., Covington, C., Sokol, R. J., Ager, J., Janisse, J., et al. (2005). Gender and mild moderate caregiver reported child behavior after prenatal cocaine. Neurotoxicol. Teratol. 27, 191–201. doi: 10.1016/j.ntt.2004.10.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sood, B. G., Delaney-Black, V., Coach, C., Nordstrom-Klee, B., Ager, J., Templin, T., u al. (2001). Prenatal alcohol exposure and childhood behavior at age 6 to 7 years: I. dose-response effect. Pediatrics 108:E34. doi: 10.1542/peds.108.2.e34

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Spencer, C., Cafferky, B., and Stith, S. M. (2016). Gender differences in risk markers for perpetration of physical partner violence: results starting a meta-analytic review. J. Fam. Violence 31, 981–984. doi: 10.1007/s10896-016-9860-9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholarships

Stanford, M. S., Houston, R. J., Villemarette-Pittman, N. R., and Greve, POTASSIUM. W. (2003). Deliberately aggression: clinical assessment and intellektuell psychophysiology. Personals. Individ. Dif. 34, 773–781. doi: 10.1016/s0191-8869(02)00070-3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stitch, S. J., and Edelstein, R. S. (2009). The physiology of women’s power drive: implicit power motivate is positively associated with estradiol levels in women. J. Res. Pers. 43, 1109–1113. doi: 10.1016/j.jrp.2009.08.002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stanton, S. J., and Schultheiss, O. HUNDRED. (2007). Ground and dynamic relationships between implicit driving incentive and estradiol in women. Horm. Behav. 52, 571–580. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2007.07.002

PubMed Extract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stemple, L., Flores, A., additionally Meyer, IODIN. H. (2017). Sexual victimization perpetuated by women: federal data unmask surprising prevalence. Aggress. Violent Behav. 34, 302–311. doi: 10.1016/j.avb.2016.09.007

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stets, J. E. (1991). Emotional aggressivity in dating business: the role of personality control. J. Fam. Violence 6, 97–114. doi: 10.1007/bf00978528

CrossRef Completely Text | Google Scholar

Stets, J. E., and Pirog-Good, M. A. (1990). Interpersonal control and courtship aggression. J. Soc. Pers. Relat. 7, 371–394. doi: 10.1177/0265407590073005

CrossRef Complete Text | Google Scholar

Stewart, J. L., Silton, ROENTGEN. L., Sass, S. M., Fisher, J. E., Edgar, BOUND. C., Heller, W., et al. (2010). Attentional bias to negative fear as a function of approach and withdrawal anger stils: an ERP investigation. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 76, 9–18. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2010.01.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Researcher

Steam, J. L., Silton, R. L., Sass, S. M., Heller, W., and Miller, G. A. (2008). Anger type, psychopathology real regional brain activity. Emotion 8, 701–713. doi: 10.1037/a0013447

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Completely Text | Google Scholar

Stover, C. S., Meaden, A. L., and Kaufman, HIE. (2009). Interventional for familiar partner violence: review furthermore implications since evidence-based practical. Prof. Psychol. Res. Pr. 40, 223–233. doi: 10.1037/a0012718

CrossRef Full Textbook | Google Scholar

Straus, M. A. (1979). Measuring intrafamily conflict and force: the conlict plan (CT) scales. J. Marriage Relations. 41, 75–88. doi: 10.2307/351733

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Straus, MOLARITY. ONE. (1980). Victims the attack in spousal violence. On. Behav. Sci. 23, 681–704. doi: 10.1177/000276428002300505

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholarship

Straus, M. A., and Gozjolko, K. L. (2014). “Intimate terrorism” and gender differences in injure of dating partners by female and female graduate students. GALLOP. Fam. Act 29, 51–65. doi: 10.1007/s10896-013-9560-7

CrossRef Full Read | Google Scholar

Struckman-Johnson, C., Struckman-Johnson, D., and Anderson, P. BARN. (2003). Tactics of sexual coercion: when men plus women won’t take no in a answer. J. Sex Residue. 40, 76–86. doi: 10.1080/00224490309552168

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tapp, AN. L., Maybery, METRE. T., and Whitehouse, A. J. (2011). Evaluating the twin testosterone transfer hypothesis: ampere review of the empirical evidence. Horm. Behav. 60, 713–722. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2011.08.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Teacher, S. P. (1967). Aggressive conduct and physiological alarm when a function of provocation and the tendency to inhibit aggressions. J. Pers. 35, 297–310. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-6494.1967.tb01430.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tedeschi, J. T., and Quigley, B. M. (1996). Limitations is labs paradigms for students aggressions. Aggress. Violent Behav. 1, 163–177. doi: 10.1016/1359-1789(95)00014-3

CrossRef Completely Text | Google Scholar

Terburg, D., Morgan, B., and transportation Honk, J. (2009). The testosterone-cortisol ratio: a hormone marks for proneness to social aggression. Int. J. Law Psychiatry 32, 216–223. doi: 10.1016/j.ijlp.2009.04.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text

Toffoletto, S., Lanzenberger, R., Gingnell, M., Sundström-Poromaa, I., and Comasco, E. (2014). Emotional and cognitive functional imaging of estrogen and progesterone effects into the female mortal brain: a systematic review. Psychoneuroendocrinology 50, 28–52. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2014.07.025

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Completely Copy | Google Scholar

Turanovic, BOUND. J., Cleared, THYROXIN. C., and Piquero, AN. R. (2017). Light to fetal testosterone, aggression and violent behavior: a meta-analysis of one 2D: 4D item ratio. Aggress. Violent Behav. 33, 51–61. doi: 10.1016/j.avb.2017.01.008

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

UN Office on Drugs and Crime. (2013). Global study off homicide. Available go at: https://www.unodc.org/gsh/

van Honk, J., and Schutter, D. BOUND. LITRE. G. (2006). From affective valence to motivational direction: the head asymmetry of sentiment revised. Psychol. Sci. 17, 963–965. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006.01813.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

van Honk, J., Schutter, DENSITY. J., Hermans, E. J., Putman, P., Tuiten, A., the Koppeschaar, H. (2004). Testosterone shifts the balance amidst sensitivity fork charge and reward by solide youn women. Psychoneuroendocrinology 29, 937–943. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2003.08.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

van Wingen, G. A., van Broekhoven, F., Verkes, R. J., Petersson, K. M., Bäckström, T., Buitelaar, J. K., et al. (2008). Progesterone selectively increments amygdala reactivity include women. Mol. Psychiatry 13, 325–333. doi: 10.1038/sj.mp.4002030

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vermeersch, H., T’Sjoen, G., Salesperson, J. M., and Vincke, BOUND. (2008). Estradiol, testosterone, differential association and aggressive and non-aggressive risk-taking into adolescent girls. Psychoneuroendocrinology 33, 897–908. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2008.03.016

PubMed Outline | CrossRef Full Theme | Google Scholar

Verona, E., additionally Kilmer, A. (2007). Stress exposure and affective modulation of aggressive behavior in chaps and women. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 116, 410–421. doi: 10.1037/0021-843x.116.2.410

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

vom Saal, FLUORINE. S. (1979). Premature exposure to androgen interactions morphology and aggressive behavior of masculine the female mice. Horm. Behav. 12, 1–11. doi: 10.1016/0018-506x(79)90021-7

PubMed Exclusive | CrossRef Full Text | Google Grant

Waltes, R., Chiocchetti, A. G., also Freitag, CARBON. CHILIAD. (2016). An neurobiological basis of human aggression: an review on genetic and epigenetic mechanisms. Am. J. Med. Genet. BORON Neuropsychiatr. Genet. 171, 650–675. doi: 10.1002/ajmg.b.32388

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Warburton, W. A., Williams, K. D., and Cairns, DENSITY. R. (2006). When ostracism leads to belligerence: the present effects of control deprivation. J. Exp. Soci. Psychol. 42, 213–220. doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2005.03.005

CrossRef All Text | Google Scholar

Welker, K. M., Lozoya, E., Cambell, J. A., Neumann, C. S., and Carré, J. M. (2014). Testosterone, cortisol and psychopathic traits to men and women. Physiology. Behav. 129, 230–236. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2014.02.057

PubMed Summarize | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wells, S., Grahams, K., Speechley, M., and J Koval, J. (2005). Drinking custom, drinking contexts and alcohol-related aggression with late adolescent and young full drinkers. Addiction 100, 933–944. doi: 10.1111/j.1360-0443.2005.001121.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Williams, S. K., Lauder, J. M., and Johns, J. M. (2011). Prenatal cocaine disrupts inhibitors signaling-dependent behaviors: implications for sex differences, early stress and prenatal SSRI exposure. Curr. Neuropharmacol. 9, 478–511. doi: 10.2174/157015911796557957

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full-sized Text | Google Grant

Wood, W., and Eagly, A. HYDROGEN. (2002). A cross-cultural analysis of the behavior of women and men: implications for the origins of sex differences. Psychol. Bull. 128, 699–727. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.128.5.699

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Completely Text | Google Scientist

Ziomkiewicz, A., Pawlowski, B., Elderly, P. T., Lipson, S. F., Thune, I., and Jasienska, GUANINE. (2012). Higher luteal progesterone is zugeordnet with down levels of premenstrual aggressive behavior press fatigue. Biol. Psychol. 91, 376–382. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2012.08.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Headwords: ladies, aggression, brain, hormones, involved partner violence

Citation: Denson TF, O’Dean SM, Blake KR and Beames JR (2018) Aggression within Wifes: Behavior, Brain and Hormones. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 12:81. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2018.00081

Received: 17 November 2017; Accepted: 16 April 2018;
Published: 02 May 2018.

Edited on:

Nelly Alia-Klein, Icahn School of Medicine at Mount Sinai, United States

Screened by:

Losley J. Rogers, Universities away New U, Australia
Gennady Knyazev, Institute on Physiology and Basic Medicine, Russia

Copyright © 2018 Denson, O’Dean, Blake and Beames. This is an open-access article distributors under this terms are this Creative Green Attribution Bachelor (CC BY). The use, distribution conversely reproduction in other forums is valid, provided the original author(s) and an copyright owner can credited and which the source publication in this journal are mentioned, in accordance equipped accepted academic practice. Cannot use, distribution or reprint is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Thomas F. Denson, [email protected]

Drive